Skip to main content
Top
Published in: BMC Public Health 1/2024

Open Access 01-12-2024 | Language Development | Research

Mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language development among toddlers: results from a large-scale survey

Authors: Signe Boe Rayce, Gunhild Tidemann Okholm, Trine Flensborg-Madsen

Published in: BMC Public Health | Issue 1/2024

Login to get access

Abstract

Background

Despite young children’s widespread use of mobile devices, little research exists on this use and its association with children’s language development. The aim of this study was to examine the associations between mobile device screen time and language comprehension and expressive language skills. An additional aim was to examine whether three factors related to the domestic learning environment modify the associations.

Methods

The study uses data from the Danish large-scale survey TRACES among two- and three-year-old children (n = 31,125). Mobile device screen time was measured as time spent on mobile devices on a normal day. Measurement of language comprehension and expressive language skills was based on subscales from the Five to Fifteen Toddlers questionnaire. Multivariable linear regression was used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language development and logistic regression to examine the risk of experiencing significant language difficulties. Joint exposure analyses were used to examine the association between child mobile device screen time and language development difficulties in combination with three other factors related to the domestic learning environment: parental education, reading to the child and child TV/PC screen time.

Results

High mobile device screen time of one hour or more per day was significantly associated with poorer language development scores and higher odds for both language comprehension difficulties (1–2 h: AOR = 1.30; ≥ 2 h: AOR = 1.42) and expressive language skills difficulties (1–2 h: AOR = 1.19; ≥ 2 h: AOR = 1.46). The results suggest that reading frequently to the child partly buffers the negative effect of high mobile device screen time on language comprehension difficulties but not on expressive language skills difficulties. No modifying effect of parental education and time spent by the child on TV/PC was found.

Conclusions

Mobile device screen time of one hour or more per day is associated with poorer language development among toddlers. Reading frequently to the child may have a buffering effect on language comprehension difficulties but not on expressive language skills difficulties.
Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Literature
1.
go back to reference Curtis PR, Frey JR, Watson CD, Hampton LH, Roberts MY. Language disorders and problem behaviors: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018;142(2). Curtis PR, Frey JR, Watson CD, Hampton LH, Roberts MY. Language disorders and problem behaviors: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018;142(2).
2.
go back to reference NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Pathways to reading: the role of oral language in the transition to reading. Dev Psychol. 2005;41(2):428–42.CrossRef NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Pathways to reading: the role of oral language in the transition to reading. Dev Psychol. 2005;41(2):428–42.CrossRef
3.
go back to reference Johnson CJ, Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB. Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2010;19:51–65.CrossRef Johnson CJ, Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB. Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. Am J Speech-Language Pathol. 2010;19:51–65.CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Raviv T, Kessenich M, Morrison FJ. A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: the role of parenting factors. Early Child Res Q. 2004;19(4):528–47.CrossRef Raviv T, Kessenich M, Morrison FJ. A mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: the role of parenting factors. Early Child Res Q. 2004;19(4):528–47.CrossRef
5.
go back to reference Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(3):244–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
6.
go back to reference Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Harrington A. Screen time use in children under 3 years old: a systematic review of correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10(1):1–10.CrossRef Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Harrington A. Screen time use in children under 3 years old: a systematic review of correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10(1):1–10.CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Haughton C, Aiken M, Cheevers C. Cyber babies: the impact of emerging technology on the developing infant. Psychol Res. 2015;5(9):504–18. Haughton C, Aiken M, Cheevers C. Cyber babies: the impact of emerging technology on the developing infant. Psychol Res. 2015;5(9):504–18.
8.
go back to reference Taylor G, Monaghan P, Westermann G. Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. J Child Media. 2018;12(1):51–65.CrossRef Taylor G, Monaghan P, Westermann G. Investigating the association between children’s screen media exposure and vocabulary size in the UK. J Child Media. 2018;12(1):51–65.CrossRef
9.
go back to reference van den Heuvel M, Ma J, Borkhoff CM, Koroshegyi C, Dai DWH, Parkin PC, et al. Mobile media device use is associated with expressive language delay in 18-month-old children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(2):99.PubMedCrossRef van den Heuvel M, Ma J, Borkhoff CM, Koroshegyi C, Dai DWH, Parkin PC, et al. Mobile media device use is associated with expressive language delay in 18-month-old children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2019;40(2):99.PubMedCrossRef
10.
go back to reference Rideout V. The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 2020. Rideout V. The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 2020.
11.
go back to reference Elias N, Sulkin I. Screen-assisted parenting: the relationship between toddlers’ screen time and parents’ use of media as a parenting tool. J Fam Issues. 2019;40(18):2801–22.CrossRef Elias N, Sulkin I. Screen-assisted parenting: the relationship between toddlers’ screen time and parents’ use of media as a parenting tool. J Fam Issues. 2019;40(18):2801–22.CrossRef
12.
go back to reference Christakis DA. The effects of infant media usage: what do we know and what should we learn? Acta Paediatr. 2009;98(1):8–16.PubMedCrossRef Christakis DA. The effects of infant media usage: what do we know and what should we learn? Acta Paediatr. 2009;98(1):8–16.PubMedCrossRef
13.
go back to reference Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665–75.PubMedCrossRef Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665–75.PubMedCrossRef
14.
go back to reference Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM, Xu D, et al. Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554–8.PubMedCrossRef Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM, Xu D, et al. Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554–8.PubMedCrossRef
15.
go back to reference Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Huberman HS, Alvir J, et al. Infant television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeconomic status households. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(5):411–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Huberman HS, Alvir J, et al. Infant television and video exposure associated with limited parent-child verbal interactions in low socioeconomic status households. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(5):411–7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
17.
go back to reference Kildare CA, Middlemiss W. Impact of parents mobile device use on parent-child interaction: a literature review. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;75:579–93.CrossRef Kildare CA, Middlemiss W. Impact of parents mobile device use on parent-child interaction: a literature review. Comput Hum Behav. 2017;75:579–93.CrossRef
18.
go back to reference Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Associations between screen-based media use and brain white matter integrity in preschool-aged children. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):e193869–193869.PubMedCrossRef Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Associations between screen-based media use and brain white matter integrity in preschool-aged children. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):e193869–193869.PubMedCrossRef
19.
go back to reference Asikainen M, Kylliäinen A, Mäkelä TE, Saarenpää-Heikkilä O, Paavonen EJ. Exposure to electronic media was negatively associated with speech and language development at 18 and 24 months. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(11):3046–53.PubMedCrossRef Asikainen M, Kylliäinen A, Mäkelä TE, Saarenpää-Heikkilä O, Paavonen EJ. Exposure to electronic media was negatively associated with speech and language development at 18 and 24 months. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(11):3046–53.PubMedCrossRef
20.
go back to reference Moon J, Cho SY, Lim SM, Roh JH, Koh MS, Kim YJ, et al. Smart device usage in early childhood is differentially associated with fine motor and language development. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108(5):903–10.PubMedCrossRef Moon J, Cho SY, Lim SM, Roh JH, Koh MS, Kim YJ, et al. Smart device usage in early childhood is differentially associated with fine motor and language development. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108(5):903–10.PubMedCrossRef
21.
go back to reference Jing M, Ye T, Kirkorian HL, Mares M. Screen media exposure and young children’s vocabulary learning and development: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2023;94(5):1398–418.PubMedCrossRef Jing M, Ye T, Kirkorian HL, Mares M. Screen media exposure and young children’s vocabulary learning and development: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2023;94(5):1398–418.PubMedCrossRef
22.
go back to reference Linebarger DL, Vaala SE. Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: an ecological perspective. Dev Rev. 2010;30(2):176–202.CrossRef Linebarger DL, Vaala SE. Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: an ecological perspective. Dev Rev. 2010;30(2):176–202.CrossRef
23.
go back to reference Radesky JS, Christakis DA. Increased screen time: implications for early childhood development and behavior. Pediatr Clin. 2016;63(5):827–39. Radesky JS, Christakis DA. Increased screen time: implications for early childhood development and behavior. Pediatr Clin. 2016;63(5):827–39.
24.
go back to reference Domoff SE, Radesky JS, Harrison K, Riley H, Lumeng JC, Miller AL. A naturalistic study of child and family screen media and mobile device use. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(2):401–10.PubMedCrossRef Domoff SE, Radesky JS, Harrison K, Riley H, Lumeng JC, Miller AL. A naturalistic study of child and family screen media and mobile device use. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(2):401–10.PubMedCrossRef
25.
go back to reference Lin H-P, Chen K-L, Chou W, Yuan K-S, Yen S-Y, Chen Y-S, et al. Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers. Infant Behav Dev. 2020;58:101424.PubMedCrossRef Lin H-P, Chen K-L, Chou W, Yuan K-S, Yen S-Y, Chen Y-S, et al. Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers. Infant Behav Dev. 2020;58:101424.PubMedCrossRef
26.
go back to reference Dowdall N, Melendez-Torres GJ, Murray L, Gardner F, Hartford L, Cooper PJ. Shared picture book reading interventions for child language development: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Child Dev. 2020;91(2):e383–99.PubMedCrossRef Dowdall N, Melendez-Torres GJ, Murray L, Gardner F, Hartford L, Cooper PJ. Shared picture book reading interventions for child language development: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Child Dev. 2020;91(2):e383–99.PubMedCrossRef
27.
go back to reference Sundqvist A, Koch F-S, Birberg Thornberg U, Barr R, Heimann M. Growing up in a digital world–digital media and the association with the child’s language development at two years of age. Front Psychol. 2021;12:569920.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sundqvist A, Koch F-S, Birberg Thornberg U, Barr R, Heimann M. Growing up in a digital world–digital media and the association with the child’s language development at two years of age. Front Psychol. 2021;12:569920.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
go back to reference Pace A, Luo R, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annu Rev Linguist. 2017;3:285–308.CrossRef Pace A, Luo R, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annu Rev Linguist. 2017;3:285–308.CrossRef
29.
go back to reference Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55–88.CrossRef Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55–88.CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Määttä S, Kaukonen R, Vepsäläinen H, Lehto E, Ylönen A, Ray C, et al. The mediating role of the home environment in relation to parental educational level and preschool children’s screen time: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(1):1–11.CrossRef Määttä S, Kaukonen R, Vepsäläinen H, Lehto E, Ylönen A, Ray C, et al. The mediating role of the home environment in relation to parental educational level and preschool children’s screen time: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(1):1–11.CrossRef
31.
go back to reference Weigel DJ, Lowman JL, Martin SS. Language development in the years before school: a comparison of developmental assets in home and child care settings. Early Child Dev Care. 2007;177(6–7):719–34.CrossRef Weigel DJ, Lowman JL, Martin SS. Language development in the years before school: a comparison of developmental assets in home and child care settings. Early Child Dev Care. 2007;177(6–7):719–34.CrossRef
34.
go back to reference Kadesjö B, Miniscalo C, Hagberg B, Haavisto A, Gillberg. Nordisk spørgeskema til vurdering af børns udvikling og adfærd. Småbørnsversion – for børn i alderen 2–5 år [Questionnaire for evaluation of development and behavior in 2–5 year old children] [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2021 Oct 27]. Available from: www.5-15.org/download. Kadesjö B, Miniscalo C, Hagberg B, Haavisto A, Gillberg. Nordisk spørgeskema til vurdering af børns udvikling og adfærd. Småbørnsversion – for børn i alderen 2–5 år [Questionnaire for evaluation of development and behavior in 2–5 year old children] [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2021 Oct 27]. Available from: www.​5-15.​org/​download.
35.
go back to reference Kadesjö B, Janois L-O, Korkman M, Mickelsson K, Strand G, Trillingsgaard A et al. Five-To-Fifteen-Revised (5-15R) [Internet]. 2017. Available from: www.5-15.org. Kadesjö B, Janois L-O, Korkman M, Mickelsson K, Strand G, Trillingsgaard A et al. Five-To-Fifteen-Revised (5-15R) [Internet]. 2017. Available from: www.5-15.org.
36.
go back to reference Lambek R, Trillingsgaard A. Elaboration, validation and standardization of the five to fifteen (FTF) questionnaire in a Danish population sample. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;38:161–70.PubMedCrossRef Lambek R, Trillingsgaard A. Elaboration, validation and standardization of the five to fifteen (FTF) questionnaire in a Danish population sample. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;38:161–70.PubMedCrossRef
37.
go back to reference Schachinger-Lorentzon U, Kadesjö B, Gillberg C, Miniscalco C. Children screening positive for language delay at 2.5 years: Language Disorder and developmental profiles. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:3267.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Schachinger-Lorentzon U, Kadesjö B, Gillberg C, Miniscalco C. Children screening positive for language delay at 2.5 years: Language Disorder and developmental profiles. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:3267.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
38.
go back to reference Hill E, Calder S, Candy C, Truscott G, Kaur J, Savage B et al. Low language capacity in childhood: a systematic review of prevalence estimates. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2023. Hill E, Calder S, Candy C, Truscott G, Kaur J, Savage B et al. Low language capacity in childhood: a systematic review of prevalence estimates. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2023.
39.
go back to reference Raaska H, Elovainio M, Sinkkonen J, Stolt S, Jalonen I, Matomäki J, et al. Adopted children’s language difficulties and their relation to symptoms of reactive attachment disorder: FinAdo study. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2013;34(3):152–60.CrossRef Raaska H, Elovainio M, Sinkkonen J, Stolt S, Jalonen I, Matomäki J, et al. Adopted children’s language difficulties and their relation to symptoms of reactive attachment disorder: FinAdo study. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2013;34(3):152–60.CrossRef
40.
go back to reference Statistics Denmark. Immigrants in Denmark [in Danish: Indvandrere i Danmark]. Copenhagen; 2008. Statistics Denmark. Immigrants in Denmark [in Danish: Indvandrere i Danmark]. Copenhagen; 2008.
41.
go back to reference Topp CW, Østergaard SD, Søndergaard S, Bech P. The WHO-5 well-being index: a systematic review of the literature. Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167–76.PubMedCrossRef Topp CW, Østergaard SD, Søndergaard S, Bech P. The WHO-5 well-being index: a systematic review of the literature. Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84(3):167–76.PubMedCrossRef
42.
go back to reference Northside Partnership Doyle O, Team UGIPE. Preparing for Life collection: Evaluation of the Preparing for Life early childhood intervention, 2008–2015. [collection]. Version 1. Dublin; 2017. Northside Partnership Doyle O, Team UGIPE. Preparing for Life collection: Evaluation of the Preparing for Life early childhood intervention, 2008–2015. [collection]. Version 1. Dublin; 2017.
43.
go back to reference Vandenbroucke JP, von Elm E, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Mulrow CD, Pocock SJ, et al. Strengthening the reporting of Observational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. Int J Surg. 2014;12(12):1500–24.PubMedCrossRef Vandenbroucke JP, von Elm E, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Mulrow CD, Pocock SJ, et al. Strengthening the reporting of Observational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. Int J Surg. 2014;12(12):1500–24.PubMedCrossRef
44.
go back to reference World Health Organization. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. World Health Organization; 2019. World Health Organization. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. World Health Organization; 2019.
45.
go back to reference American Academy of Pediatrics. Children and media tips from the American Academy of Pediatrics. 2018. American Academy of Pediatrics. Children and media tips from the American Academy of Pediatrics. 2018.
46.
go back to reference Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern epidemiology. Volume 3. Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Philadelphia; 2008. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern epidemiology. Volume 3. Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Philadelphia; 2008.
47.
go back to reference Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Confidence interval estimation of interaction. Epidemiology. 1992;452–6. Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Confidence interval estimation of interaction. Epidemiology. 1992;452–6.
48.
go back to reference McArthur BA, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen time and developmental and behavioral outcomes for preschool children. Pediatr Res. 2022;91(6):1616–21.PubMedCrossRef McArthur BA, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen time and developmental and behavioral outcomes for preschool children. Pediatr Res. 2022;91(6):1616–21.PubMedCrossRef
49.
go back to reference Vanderloo LM, Janus M, Omand JA, Keown-Stoneman CDG, Borkhoff CM, Duku E, et al. Children’s screen use and school readiness at 4–6 years: prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–11.CrossRef Vanderloo LM, Janus M, Omand JA, Keown-Stoneman CDG, Borkhoff CM, Duku E, et al. Children’s screen use and school readiness at 4–6 years: prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–11.CrossRef
50.
go back to reference McArthur BA, Eirich R, McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen use relates to decreased offline enrichment activities. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(3):896–8.PubMedCrossRef McArthur BA, Eirich R, McDonald S, Tough S, Madigan S. Screen use relates to decreased offline enrichment activities. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(3):896–8.PubMedCrossRef
51.
go back to reference Radesky JS, Eisenberg S, Kistin CJ, Gross J, Block G, Zuckerman B, et al. Overstimulated consumers or next-generation learners? Parent tensions about child mobile technology use. Ann Fam Med. 2016;14(6):503–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Radesky JS, Eisenberg S, Kistin CJ, Gross J, Block G, Zuckerman B, et al. Overstimulated consumers or next-generation learners? Parent tensions about child mobile technology use. Ann Fam Med. 2016;14(6):503–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
52.
go back to reference Brown A, Smolenaers E. Parents’ interpretations of screen time recommendations for children younger than 2 years. J Fam Issues. 2018;39(2):406–29.CrossRef Brown A, Smolenaers E. Parents’ interpretations of screen time recommendations for children younger than 2 years. J Fam Issues. 2018;39(2):406–29.CrossRef
53.
go back to reference Levine LE, Waite BM, Bowman LL, Kachinsky K. Mobile media use by infants and toddlers. Comput Hum Behav. 2019;94:92–9.CrossRef Levine LE, Waite BM, Bowman LL, Kachinsky K. Mobile media use by infants and toddlers. Comput Hum Behav. 2019;94:92–9.CrossRef
54.
go back to reference Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L, Melstein Damast A. Responsive parenting in the second year: specific influences on children’s language and play. Early Dev Parent Int J Res Pract. 1996;5(4):173–83.CrossRef Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L, Melstein Damast A. Responsive parenting in the second year: specific influences on children’s language and play. Early Dev Parent Int J Res Pract. 1996;5(4):173–83.CrossRef
55.
go back to reference Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Language development and intelligence in midlife. Br J Dev Psychol. 2019;37(2):269–83.PubMedCrossRef Flensborg-Madsen T, Mortensen EL. Language development and intelligence in midlife. Br J Dev Psychol. 2019;37(2):269–83.PubMedCrossRef
Metadata
Title
Mobile device screen time is associated with poorer language development among toddlers: results from a large-scale survey
Authors
Signe Boe Rayce
Gunhild Tidemann Okholm
Trine Flensborg-Madsen
Publication date
01-12-2024
Publisher
BioMed Central
Published in
BMC Public Health / Issue 1/2024
Electronic ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18447-4

Other articles of this Issue 1/2024

BMC Public Health 1/2024 Go to the issue