Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 1/2011

01-03-2011 | Research Article

Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 1/2011

Login to get access

Abstract

Antisaccades require the suppression of a stimulus-driven response (i.e., response suppression) and the computation of a movement plan mirror-symmetrical to the location of a target (i.e., vector inversion). The goal of the present study was to determine whether response suppression, vector inversion or both contribute to previously reported differences in the online control of pro- and antisaccades (Heath in Exp Brain Res 203:743–752, 2010a). Pro- and antisaccades were completed in separate blocks (i.e., blocked schedule) and a block wherein the spatial relation between stimulus and response was provided at response cuing (i.e., random schedule). Notably, the random schedule provides a relative means for equating response suppression across pro- and antisaccades. To examine online trajectory amendments, we computed the proportion of variance (R 2 values) explained by the spatial location of the eye at early, middle and late stages of saccade trajectories relative to the saccade’s ultimate endpoint. The basis for this analysis is that between-task differences in R 2 magnitudes reflect differences in the use of feedback for online trajectory amendments: small R 2 values represent a trajectory supported via online control whereas larger R 2 values reflect a reduction in online control. Results show that antisaccades yielded larger R 2 values than prosaccades from early to late stages of saccade trajectories, and this finding was observed regardless of whether or not tasks were equated for response suppression. Thus, we propose that the intentional nature of vector inversion disrupts the normally online control of saccades and renders a mode of control that is not optimized to support error-reducing trajectory amendments.
Footnotes
1
As indicated in the “Methods”, trials involving a direction error were placed back in the random trial matrix and rerun. Thus, the number of direction errors reported here represents the average number of error trials (per participant) in each of the blocked and random schedules.
 
2
MT data are reported in order to show that differences in endpoint accuracy are unrelated to a speed/accuracy trade-off and to demonstrate that between-task and between-schedule differences in R 2 values are unrelated to movement time differences.
 
3
In a previous study, we demonstrated that saccades directed left or right of a central fixation did not influence the spatiotemporal parameters of pro- and antisaccade trajectories (Heath et al. 2010a; see also West et al. 2009). For that reason, space (i.e., targets in left vs. right space) was not included in our ANOVA model.
 
4
As a matter of course, we do not compute multi-directional post hoc contrasts. That said, we thought it important to demonstrate that blocked schedule prosaccades elicited faster RTs than any of the other experimental conditions. Thus, comparison of prosaccades across feedback schedules revealed that blocked schedule ones were reliably faster than their random schedule counterparts (t(11) = 6.84, P < 0.001).
 
5
Examination of the ANOVA summary for R 2 values revealed F-ratios less than one for schedule, schedule by task, and time by schedule by task. The null effect of blocked and random schedules on saccade trajectories is therefore not tied to an inadequate replication sample size.
 
Literature
go back to reference Becker W, Fuchs AF (1969) Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vision Res 9:1247–1258CrossRefPubMed Becker W, Fuchs AF (1969) Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vision Res 9:1247–1258CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Binsted G, Heath M (2004) Can the motor system utilize a stored representation to control movement? Behav Brain Sci 27:25–27CrossRef Binsted G, Heath M (2004) Can the motor system utilize a stored representation to control movement? Behav Brain Sci 27:25–27CrossRef
go back to reference Binsted G, Brownell K, Vorontsova Z, Heath M, Saucier D (2007) Visuomotor system uses target features unavailable to conscious awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:12669–12672CrossRefPubMed Binsted G, Brownell K, Vorontsova Z, Heath M, Saucier D (2007) Visuomotor system uses target features unavailable to conscious awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:12669–12672CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bridgeman B, Lewis S, Heit G, Nagle M (1979) Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 5:692–700CrossRefPubMed Bridgeman B, Lewis S, Heit G, Nagle M (1979) Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 5:692–700CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brown MR, DeSouza JF, Goltz HC, Ford K, Menon RS, Goodale MA, Everling S (2004) Comparison of memory- and visually guided saccades using event- related fMRI. J Neurophysiol 91:873–889CrossRefPubMed Brown MR, DeSouza JF, Goltz HC, Ford K, Menon RS, Goodale MA, Everling S (2004) Comparison of memory- and visually guided saccades using event- related fMRI. J Neurophysiol 91:873–889CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Brown MR, Goltz HC, Vilis T, Ford KA, Everling S (2006) Inhibition and generation of saccades: rapid event-related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials. Neuroimage 33:644–659CrossRefPubMed Brown MR, Goltz HC, Vilis T, Ford KA, Everling S (2006) Inhibition and generation of saccades: rapid event-related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials. Neuroimage 33:644–659CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Connolly JD, Goodale MA, Desouza JF, Menon RS, Vilis T (2000) A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. J Neurophysiol 84:1645–1655PubMed Connolly JD, Goodale MA, Desouza JF, Menon RS, Vilis T (2000) A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. J Neurophysiol 84:1645–1655PubMed
go back to reference Dafoe JM, Armstrong IT, Munoz DP (2007) The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Exp Brain Res 179:563–570CrossRefPubMed Dafoe JM, Armstrong IT, Munoz DP (2007) The influence of stimulus direction and eccentricity on pro- and anti-saccades in humans. Exp Brain Res 179:563–570CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Deubel H (1995) Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements. Vision Res 35:3529–3540CrossRefPubMed Deubel H (1995) Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements. Vision Res 35:3529–3540CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Doma H, Hallett PE (1988) Dependence of saccadic eye-movements on stimulus luminance, and an effect of task. Vision Res 28:915–924CrossRefPubMed Doma H, Hallett PE (1988) Dependence of saccadic eye-movements on stimulus luminance, and an effect of task. Vision Res 28:915–924CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M (1999) The control of goal-directed limb movements: correcting errors in the trajectory. Hum Mov Sci 18:121–136CrossRef Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M (1999) The control of goal-directed limb movements: correcting errors in the trajectory. Hum Mov Sci 18:121–136CrossRef
go back to reference Evdokimidis I, Tsekou H, Smyrnis N (2006) The mirror antisaccade task: direction- amplitude interaction and spatial accuracy characteristics. Exp Brain Res 174:304–311CrossRefPubMed Evdokimidis I, Tsekou H, Smyrnis N (2006) The mirror antisaccade task: direction- amplitude interaction and spatial accuracy characteristics. Exp Brain Res 174:304–311CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Everling S, Spantekow A, Krappmann P, Flohr H (1998) Event-related potentials associated with correct and incorrect responses in a cued antisaccade task. Exp Brain Res 118:27–34CrossRefPubMed Everling S, Spantekow A, Krappmann P, Flohr H (1998) Event-related potentials associated with correct and incorrect responses in a cued antisaccade task. Exp Brain Res 118:27–34CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer B, Weber H (1992) Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man. Exp Brain Res 89:415–424CrossRefPubMed Fischer B, Weber H (1992) Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man. Exp Brain Res 89:415–424CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391CrossRefPubMed Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ford KA, Goltz HC, Brown MR, Everling S (2005) Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. J Neurophysiol 94:429–440CrossRefPubMed Ford KA, Goltz HC, Brown MR, Everling S (2005) Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. J Neurophysiol 94:429–440CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gaveau V, Martin O, Prablanc C, Pélisson D, Urquizar C, Desmurget M (2003) Online modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals. Neuroreport 14:875–878CrossRefPubMed Gaveau V, Martin O, Prablanc C, Pélisson D, Urquizar C, Desmurget M (2003) Online modification of saccadic eye movements by retinal signals. Neuroreport 14:875–878CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gaveau V, Pélisson D, Blangero A, Urquizar C, Prablanc C, Vighetto A, Pisella L (2008) Saccade control and eye-hand coordination in optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia 46:475–486CrossRefPubMed Gaveau V, Pélisson D, Blangero A, Urquizar C, Prablanc C, Vighetto A, Pisella L (2008) Saccade control and eye-hand coordination in optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia 46:475–486CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Goodale MA (2010) Transforming vision into action. Vision Res (in press) Goodale MA (2010) Transforming vision into action. Vision Res (in press)
go back to reference Goodale MA, Pelisson D, Prablanc C (1986) Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320:748–750CrossRefPubMed Goodale MA, Pelisson D, Prablanc C (1986) Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320:748–750CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hallett PE (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res 18:1279–1296CrossRefPubMed Hallett PE (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Res 18:1279–1296CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heath M (2005) Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. Mot Control 9:281–311 Heath M (2005) Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. Mot Control 9:281–311
go back to reference Heath M, Rival C, Binsted G (2004a) Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion. Exp Brain Res 158:378–384CrossRefPubMed Heath M, Rival C, Binsted G (2004a) Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion. Exp Brain Res 158:378–384CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heath M, Westwood DA, Binsted G (2004b) The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. Mot Control 8:76–106 Heath M, Westwood DA, Binsted G (2004b) The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. Mot Control 8:76–106
go back to reference Heath M, Maraj A, Gradkowski A, Binsted G (2009a) Anti-pointing is mediated by a perceptual bias of target location in left and right visual space. Exp Brain Res 192:275–286CrossRefPubMed Heath M, Maraj A, Gradkowski A, Binsted G (2009a) Anti-pointing is mediated by a perceptual bias of target location in left and right visual space. Exp Brain Res 192:275–286CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heath M, Maraj A, Maddigan M, Binsted G (2009b) The antipointing task: vector inversion is supported by a perceptual estimate of visual space. J Mot Behav 41:383–392CrossRefPubMed Heath M, Maraj A, Maddigan M, Binsted G (2009b) The antipointing task: vector inversion is supported by a perceptual estimate of visual space. J Mot Behav 41:383–392CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heath M, Dunham K, Binsted G, Godbolt B (2010a) Antisaccades exhibit diminished online control relative to prosaccades. Exp Brain Res 203:743–752CrossRefPubMed Heath M, Dunham K, Binsted G, Godbolt B (2010a) Antisaccades exhibit diminished online control relative to prosaccades. Exp Brain Res 203:743–752CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Heath M, Neely KA, Krigolson O, Binsted G (2010b) Memory-guided reaching: what the visuomotor system knows and how long it knows it. In: Elliott D, Khan M (eds) Vision and goal-directed movement: neurobahevioral perspectives. Human Kinetics, Champaign, pp 79–96 Heath M, Neely KA, Krigolson O, Binsted G (2010b) Memory-guided reaching: what the visuomotor system knows and how long it knows it. In: Elliott D, Khan M (eds) Vision and goal-directed movement: neurobahevioral perspectives. Human Kinetics, Champaign, pp 79–96
go back to reference Krappmann P, Everling S, Flohr H (1998) Accuracy of visually and memory-guided antisaccades in man. Vision Res 38:2979–2985CrossRefPubMed Krappmann P, Everling S, Flohr H (1998) Accuracy of visually and memory-guided antisaccades in man. Vision Res 38:2979–2985CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Krigolson O, Clark N, Heath M, Binsted G (2007) The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. Spat Vis 20:317–336CrossRefPubMed Krigolson O, Clark N, Heath M, Binsted G (2007) The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. Spat Vis 20:317–336CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Maraj A, Heath M (2010) Antipointing: perception-based visual information renders an offline mode of control. Exp Brain Res 202:55–64CrossRefPubMed Maraj A, Heath M (2010) Antipointing: perception-based visual information renders an offline mode of control. Exp Brain Res 202:55–64CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference McDowell JE, Dyckman KA, Austin BP, Clementz BA (2008) Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: evidence from studies of humans. Brain Cogn 68:255–270CrossRefPubMed McDowell JE, Dyckman KA, Austin BP, Clementz BA (2008) Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades: evidence from studies of humans. Brain Cogn 68:255–270CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152CrossRefPubMed Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Moon SY, Barton JJ, Mikulski S, Polli FE, Cain MS, Vangel M, Hämäläinen MS, Manoach DS (2007) Where left becomes right: a magnetoencephalographic study of sensorimotor transformation for antisaccades. Neuroimage 36:1313–1323CrossRefPubMed Moon SY, Barton JJ, Mikulski S, Polli FE, Cain MS, Vangel M, Hämäläinen MS, Manoach DS (2007) Where left becomes right: a magnetoencephalographic study of sensorimotor transformation for antisaccades. Neuroimage 36:1313–1323CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228CrossRefPubMed Munoz DP, Everling S (2004) Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:218–228CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Olk B, Kingstone A (2003) Why are antisaccades slower than prosaccades? A novel finding using a new paradigm. Neuroreport 14:151–155CrossRefPubMed Olk B, Kingstone A (2003) Why are antisaccades slower than prosaccades? A novel finding using a new paradigm. Neuroreport 14:151–155CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pedhazur EJ (1997) Multiple regression in behavioral research: explanation and prediction. Harcourt Brace Publishers, Orlando, FL Pedhazur EJ (1997) Multiple regression in behavioral research: explanation and prediction. Harcourt Brace Publishers, Orlando, FL
go back to reference Pisella L, Gréa H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Desmurget M, Rode G, Boisson D, Rossetti Y (2000) An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 3:729–736CrossRefPubMed Pisella L, Gréa H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Desmurget M, Rode G, Boisson D, Rossetti Y (2000) An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 3:729–736CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Prablanc C, Martin O (1992) Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. J Neurophysiol 67:455–469PubMed Prablanc C, Martin O (1992) Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. J Neurophysiol 67:455–469PubMed
go back to reference Robinson DA (1964) The mechanics of human saccadic eye movement. J Physiol 174:245–264PubMed Robinson DA (1964) The mechanics of human saccadic eye movement. J Physiol 174:245–264PubMed
go back to reference Rossetti Y, Revol P, McIntosh R, Pisella L, Rode G, Danckert J, Tilikete C, Dijkerman HC, Boisson D, Vighetto A, Michel F, Milner AD (2005) Visually guided reaching: bilateral posterior parietal lesions cause a switch from fast visuomotor to slow cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 43:162–177CrossRefPubMed Rossetti Y, Revol P, McIntosh R, Pisella L, Rode G, Danckert J, Tilikete C, Dijkerman HC, Boisson D, Vighetto A, Michel F, Milner AD (2005) Visually guided reaching: bilateral posterior parietal lesions cause a switch from fast visuomotor to slow cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 43:162–177CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sparks DL, Holland R, Guthrie BL (1976) Size and distribution of movement fields in the monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res 113:21–34CrossRefPubMed Sparks DL, Holland R, Guthrie BL (1976) Size and distribution of movement fields in the monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res 113:21–34CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference West GL, Welsh TN, Pratt J (2009) Saccadic trajectories receive online correction: evidence for a feedback-based system of oculomotor control. J Mot Behav 41:117–127CrossRefPubMed West GL, Welsh TN, Pratt J (2009) Saccadic trajectories receive online correction: evidence for a feedback-based system of oculomotor control. J Mot Behav 41:117–127CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (1972) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. IV. Effects of lesions on eye movements. J Neurophysiol 35:587–596PubMed Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (1972) Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. IV. Effects of lesions on eye movements. J Neurophysiol 35:587–596PubMed
go back to reference Zhang M, Barash S (2000) Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades. Nature 408:971–975CrossRefPubMed Zhang M, Barash S (2000) Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades. Nature 408:971–975CrossRefPubMed
Metadata
Title
Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades
Publication date
01-03-2011
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 1/2011
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2525-7

Other articles of this Issue 1/2011

Experimental Brain Research 1/2011 Go to the issue