Skip to main content
Top
Published in: The European Journal of Health Economics 9/2023

04-01-2023 | Original Paper

The social value of gambling: surplus estimates by gambling types for France

Authors: Maxence Miéra, Sophie Massin, Vincent Eroukmanoff

Published in: The European Journal of Health Economics | Issue 9/2023

Login to get access

Abstract

We estimate the social surplus of gambling in France by adding three components: consumer surplus, producer surplus and taxation revenue. To estimate consumer surplus, we use the rational benchmark approach, which attributes a loss of welfare (i.e. a negative surplus) to problem gamblers depending on their level of excess spending compared with recreational gamblers. Using data for the year 2019 and considering only legal gambling, we find that the consumer surplus is negative for the gambling activity as a whole. When we add the producer surplus and the taxation revenue to the consumer surplus, we find that the social surplus is more likely to be negative, ranging from − 45 billion euros in the pessimistic scenario to + 6 billion euros in the optimistic scenario. There are, however, important differences between gambling types. The social surplus is negative in all scenarios for poker and sports betting. Conversely, it is positive in all scenarios for draw lotteries and scratch cards.
Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
The concepts of price and quantity are a bit peculiar in the gambling sector. We will explain in more detail what they mean and how we measure them in "Gambling spending and revenue at the aggregate level".
 
2
The price uniqueness implicitly requires that the expected loss rate is identical for all gamblers and homogeneous within each gambling type, which is a simplifying working assumption.
 
3
See Soullier et al. [12] for a presentation of the Health Barometer.
 
4
This issue substantially reflects the distinct nature of a public health approach and a clinical one. The matter is rather complex, and the overview is quite schematic. See Delfabbro and King [21] and Browne and Rockloff [16] on the relevance of the prevention paradox for gambling, and Browne and Rockloff [22] and Delfabbro and King [23], on the difference between gambling-related harms and gambling disorders.
 
5
The measure of problem gambler surplus (Eq. 6) is non-linear. Thus, the measurement obtained in a group with a PGSI score of 3–27 is different from that obtained by summing the surplus of subgroups with a PGSI score of 3–7 and 8–27. It then seems reasonable to separate the two groups in the calculation to preserve their characteristics rather than assigning them those of an aggregate group.
 
6
This way of defining gambling spending is equivalent to that of the GGR (which removes winnings from bets). The issues of proper understanding of the question and reliability of self-reported data are addressed in the discussion.
 
7
Note that this method is not usable for slot machines, poker, and other table games because of the small number of regular recreational gamblers. As pointed out by the Productivity Commission ([1], vol. 3, p. C19) for the casino table game category: “even ‘enthusiastic’ recreational players, appear not to play weekly”. We then use the median spending of all recreational gamblers rather than regular recreational gamblers for these games.
 
8
The Française des Jeux (FDJ) has a monopoly on offline and online lottery games and offline sports betting. The Pari Mutuel Urbain (PMU) has a monopoly on offline horse racing betting. Slot machines and table games are authorized only in land-based casinos, which are subject to an operating licence from the Ministry of the Interior. Online sports and horse racing betting and poker are the only games officially open to competition.
 
9
See Tirole ([26], p. 66 et seq.) on monopoly pricing behaviour.
 
10
I.e. an approach where the producer surplus is measured as: \(PS = NGR - VC = \Pi + FC\), where \(VC\) is the variable costs, \(\Pi\) is the economic profit, and \(FC\) is the fixed costs. To our knowledge, there is no other available estimate of producer surplus in the literature.
 
11
See Table 12 in the Appendix for details of the correspondence.
 
12
Details of gains and losses for each category of problem gamblers are provided in the Appendix (Table 13). The loss on excess spending exceeds the gain on recreational spending for each category of problem gamblers and each type of gambling. As a result, the surplus is negative for each type of gambling and each category of problem gamblers.
 
13
The shift in the threshold trivially changes the categories of recreational and problem gamblers. It also modifies the median spending of recreational gamblers (regular or not) and the share of recreational spending for problem gamblers. Note further that we compute the surpluses of problem gamblers with PGSI scores of 1–2, 3–7, and 8–27 separately before adding them when the threshold is set at 1 + .
 
14
Note that the loss in consumer surplus exceeds the GGR for the gambling type with a negative social surplus in the baseline scenario Table 6. It follows that the estimate of producer surplus does not impact the sign of the social surplus in the baseline scenario since the taxation revenue plus the producer surplus is necessarily inferior to the GGR.
 
Literature
2.
go back to reference Bernheim, B.D., Rangel, A.: Addiction and cue-triggered decision processes. Am. Econ. Rev. 94(5), 1558–1590 (2004)CrossRefPubMed Bernheim, B.D., Rangel, A.: Addiction and cue-triggered decision processes. Am. Econ. Rev. 94(5), 1558–1590 (2004)CrossRefPubMed
3.
go back to reference Gruber, J., Köszegi, B.: Is addiction “rational”? Theory and evidence. Q. J. Econ. 116(4), 1261–1303 (2001)CrossRef Gruber, J., Köszegi, B.: Is addiction “rational”? Theory and evidence. Q. J. Econ. 116(4), 1261–1303 (2001)CrossRef
4.
go back to reference Lesieur, H. R., Anderson, C.: Results of a survey of gamblers Anonymous members in Illinois. Illinois Council on Problem and Compulsive Gambling. Mimeographed (1995) Lesieur, H. R., Anderson, C.: Results of a survey of gamblers Anonymous members in Illinois. Illinois Council on Problem and Compulsive Gambling. Mimeographed (1995)
5.
go back to reference Thompson, W.N., Gazel, R., Rickman, D.: The social costs of gambling in Wisconsin. Wisconsin Policy Res. Inst. Rep. 9(6), 1–44 (1996) Thompson, W.N., Gazel, R., Rickman, D.: The social costs of gambling in Wisconsin. Wisconsin Policy Res. Inst. Rep. 9(6), 1–44 (1996)
6.
go back to reference Gerstein, D., Murphy, S., Toce, M., Hoffman, J., Palmer, A., Johnson, R., Larison, C., Chuchro, L., Buie, T., Engelman, L., Hill, M.A.: Gambling impact and behavior study: report to the National Gambling Impact Study Commission. National Opinion Research Center (1999) Gerstein, D., Murphy, S., Toce, M., Hoffman, J., Palmer, A., Johnson, R., Larison, C., Chuchro, L., Buie, T., Engelman, L., Hill, M.A.: Gambling impact and behavior study: report to the National Gambling Impact Study Commission. National Opinion Research Center (1999)
7.
go back to reference Thorley, C., Stirling, A., Huynh, E.: Cards on the table: the cost to government associated with people who are problem gamblers in Britain. Institute for Public Policy Research, London (2016) Thorley, C., Stirling, A., Huynh, E.: Cards on the table: the cost to government associated with people who are problem gamblers in Britain. Institute for Public Policy Research, London (2016)
8.
go back to reference Fiedler, I.: Glücksspiele: Eine verhaltens- und gesundheitsökonomische Analyse mit rechtspolitischen Empfehlungen. Peter Lang, Frankfurt am Main (2016)CrossRef Fiedler, I.: Glücksspiele: Eine verhaltens- und gesundheitsökonomische Analyse mit rechtspolitischen Empfehlungen. Peter Lang, Frankfurt am Main (2016)CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Massin, S., Miéra, M.: Measuring consumer surplus in the case of addiction: a re-examination of the rational benchmark algebra. Econ. Bull. 40(4), 3171–3181 (2020) Massin, S., Miéra, M.: Measuring consumer surplus in the case of addiction: a re-examination of the rational benchmark algebra. Econ. Bull. 40(4), 3171–3181 (2020)
10.
go back to reference Gruber, J.: Smoking’s internalities. Regulation 25(4), 52–58 (2002) Gruber, J.: Smoking’s internalities. Regulation 25(4), 52–58 (2002)
11.
go back to reference Ferris, J., Wynne, H.: The Canadian problem gambling index. Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse, Ottawa, ON (2001) Ferris, J., Wynne, H.: The Canadian problem gambling index. Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse, Ottawa, ON (2001)
12.
go back to reference Soullier, N., Richard, J-B., Gautier, A.: Baromètre santé 2019. Méthode d'enquête. Objectifs, contexte de mise en place et protocole. Santé publique France (2021) Soullier, N., Richard, J-B., Gautier, A.: Baromètre santé 2019. Méthode d'enquête. Objectifs, contexte de mise en place et protocole. Santé publique France (2021)
13.
go back to reference Currie, S.R., Hodgins, D.C., Casey, D.M.: Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories. J. Gambl. Stud. 29(2), 311–327 (2013)CrossRefPubMed Currie, S.R., Hodgins, D.C., Casey, D.M.: Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories. J. Gambl. Stud. 29(2), 311–327 (2013)CrossRefPubMed
14.
go back to reference Stone, C.A., Romild, U., Abbott, M., Yeung, K., Billi, R., Volberg, R.: Effects of different screening and scoring thresholds on PGSI gambling risk segments. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 13(1), 82–102 (2015)CrossRef Stone, C.A., Romild, U., Abbott, M., Yeung, K., Billi, R., Volberg, R.: Effects of different screening and scoring thresholds on PGSI gambling risk segments. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 13(1), 82–102 (2015)CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Browne, M., Rawat, V., Greer, N., Langham, E., Rockloff, M., Hanley, C.: What is the harm? Applying a public health methodology to measure the impact of gambling problems and harm on quality of life. J. Gambl. Issues 36, 28–50 (2017) Browne, M., Rawat, V., Greer, N., Langham, E., Rockloff, M., Hanley, C.: What is the harm? Applying a public health methodology to measure the impact of gambling problems and harm on quality of life. J. Gambl. Issues 36, 28–50 (2017)
16.
17.
go back to reference Li, E., Browne, M., Rawat, V., Langham, E., Rockloff, M.: Breaking bad: comparing gambling harms among gamblers and affected others. J. Gambl. Stud. 33(1), 223–248 (2017)CrossRefPubMed Li, E., Browne, M., Rawat, V., Langham, E., Rockloff, M.: Breaking bad: comparing gambling harms among gamblers and affected others. J. Gambl. Stud. 33(1), 223–248 (2017)CrossRefPubMed
18.
go back to reference Ladouceur, R., Jacques, C., Chevalier, S., Sevigny, S., Hamel, D.: Prevalence of pathological gambling in Quebec in 2002. Can. J. Psychiatry 50(8), 451–456 (2005)CrossRefPubMed Ladouceur, R., Jacques, C., Chevalier, S., Sevigny, S., Hamel, D.: Prevalence of pathological gambling in Quebec in 2002. Can. J. Psychiatry 50(8), 451–456 (2005)CrossRefPubMed
19.
go back to reference Samuelsson, E., Wennberg, P., Sundqvist, K.: Gamblers’(mis-) interpretations of Problem Gambling Severity Index items: ambiguities in qualitative accounts from the Swedish Longitudinal Gambling Study. Nordic Stud. Alcohol Drugs 36(2), 140–160 (2019)CrossRef Samuelsson, E., Wennberg, P., Sundqvist, K.: Gamblers’(mis-) interpretations of Problem Gambling Severity Index items: ambiguities in qualitative accounts from the Swedish Longitudinal Gambling Study. Nordic Stud. Alcohol Drugs 36(2), 140–160 (2019)CrossRef
20.
go back to reference Williams, R.J., Volberg, R.A.: The classification accuracy of four problem gambling assessment instruments in population research. Int. Gambl. Stud. 14(1), 15–28 (2014)CrossRef Williams, R.J., Volberg, R.A.: The classification accuracy of four problem gambling assessment instruments in population research. Int. Gambl. Stud. 14(1), 15–28 (2014)CrossRef
21.
go back to reference Delfabbro, P., King, D.: Prevention paradox logic and problem gambling: Does low-risk gambling impose a greater burden of harm than high-risk gambling? J. Behav. Addict. 6(2), 163–167 (2017)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Delfabbro, P., King, D.: Prevention paradox logic and problem gambling: Does low-risk gambling impose a greater burden of harm than high-risk gambling? J. Behav. Addict. 6(2), 163–167 (2017)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
22.
go back to reference Browne, M., Rockloff, M.J.: The dangers of conflating gambling-related harm with disordered gambling: Commentary on: Prevention paradox logic and problem gambling (Delfabbro & King, 2017). J. Behav. Addict. 6(3), 317–320 (2017)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Browne, M., Rockloff, M.J.: The dangers of conflating gambling-related harm with disordered gambling: Commentary on: Prevention paradox logic and problem gambling (Delfabbro & King, 2017). J. Behav. Addict. 6(3), 317–320 (2017)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
go back to reference Delfabbro, P., King, D.L.: Don’t say the ‘P’Word: problem gambling is more than harm. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 18(3), 835–843 (2020)CrossRef Delfabbro, P., King, D.L.: Don’t say the ‘P’Word: problem gambling is more than harm. Int. J. Ment. Heal. Addict. 18(3), 835–843 (2020)CrossRef
24.
go back to reference Currie, S.R., Miller, N., Hodgins, D.C., Wang, J.: Defining a threshold of harm from gambling for population health surveillance research. Int. Gambl. Stud. 9(1), 19–38 (2009)CrossRef Currie, S.R., Miller, N., Hodgins, D.C., Wang, J.: Defining a threshold of harm from gambling for population health surveillance research. Int. Gambl. Stud. 9(1), 19–38 (2009)CrossRef
25.
go back to reference Gallet, C.: Gambling demand: a meta-analysis of the price elasticity. J. Gambl. Bus. Econ. 9(1), 13–22 (2015)CrossRef Gallet, C.: Gambling demand: a meta-analysis of the price elasticity. J. Gambl. Bus. Econ. 9(1), 13–22 (2015)CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Tirole, J.: The theory of industrial organization. MIT Press (1988) Tirole, J.: The theory of industrial organization. MIT Press (1988)
27.
go back to reference Allen consulting group. Social and economic impact study of gambling in Tasmania. Volume 1: Gambling industry and economic impacts. Prepared for the Tasmanian Government Department of Treasury and finance (2011) Allen consulting group. Social and economic impact study of gambling in Tasmania. Volume 1: Gambling industry and economic impacts. Prepared for the Tasmanian Government Department of Treasury and finance (2011)
28.
go back to reference Chambers, K.G.: Gambling for profit. University of Toronto Press (2017) Chambers, K.G.: Gambling for profit. University of Toronto Press (2017)
29.
go back to reference Fiedler, I., Kairouz, S., Costes, J.M., Weißmüller, K.S.: Gambling spending and its concentration on problem gamblers. J. Bus. Res. 98, 82–91 (2019)CrossRef Fiedler, I., Kairouz, S., Costes, J.M., Weißmüller, K.S.: Gambling spending and its concentration on problem gamblers. J. Bus. Res. 98, 82–91 (2019)CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Slutske, W.S.: Longitudinal studies of gambling behavior. In: Smith, G., Hodgins, D.C., Williams, R.J. (eds.) Research and measurement issues in gambling studies, pp. 127–154. Academic Press, Burlington, MA (2007) Slutske, W.S.: Longitudinal studies of gambling behavior. In: Smith, G., Hodgins, D.C., Williams, R.J. (eds.) Research and measurement issues in gambling studies, pp. 127–154. Academic Press, Burlington, MA (2007)
31.
go back to reference Toce-Gerstein, M., Gerstein, D.R., Volberg, R.A.: A hierarchy of gambling disorders in the community. Addiction 98, 1661–1672 (2003)CrossRefPubMed Toce-Gerstein, M., Gerstein, D.R., Volberg, R.A.: A hierarchy of gambling disorders in the community. Addiction 98, 1661–1672 (2003)CrossRefPubMed
32.
go back to reference Wood, R.T., Williams, R.J.: ‘How much money do you spend on gambling?’ The comparative validity of question wordings used to assess gambling expenditure. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 10(1), 63–77 (2007)CrossRef Wood, R.T., Williams, R.J.: ‘How much money do you spend on gambling?’ The comparative validity of question wordings used to assess gambling expenditure. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 10(1), 63–77 (2007)CrossRef
33.
go back to reference Heirene, R.M., Wang, A., Gainsbury, S.M.: Accuracy of self-reported gambling frequency and outcomes: comparisons with account data. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 36(4), 333–346 (2022)CrossRefPubMed Heirene, R.M., Wang, A., Gainsbury, S.M.: Accuracy of self-reported gambling frequency and outcomes: comparisons with account data. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 36(4), 333–346 (2022)CrossRefPubMed
34.
go back to reference Auer, M., Griffiths, M.D.: Self-reported losses versus actual losses in online gambling: an empirical study. J. Gambl. Stud. 33(3), 795–806 (2017)CrossRefPubMed Auer, M., Griffiths, M.D.: Self-reported losses versus actual losses in online gambling: an empirical study. J. Gambl. Stud. 33(3), 795–806 (2017)CrossRefPubMed
35.
go back to reference Braverman, J., Tom, M.A., Shaffer, H.J.: Accuracy of self-reported versus actual online gambling wins and losses. Psychol. Assess. 26(3), 865–877 (2014)CrossRefPubMed Braverman, J., Tom, M.A., Shaffer, H.J.: Accuracy of self-reported versus actual online gambling wins and losses. Psychol. Assess. 26(3), 865–877 (2014)CrossRefPubMed
37.
go back to reference James, R.J., O’Malley, C., Tunney, R.J.: Why are some games more addictive than others: the effects of timing and payoff on perseverance in a slot machine game. Front. Psychol. 7, 46 (2016)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral James, R.J., O’Malley, C., Tunney, R.J.: Why are some games more addictive than others: the effects of timing and payoff on perseverance in a slot machine game. Front. Psychol. 7, 46 (2016)CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadata
Title
The social value of gambling: surplus estimates by gambling types for France
Authors
Maxence Miéra
Sophie Massin
Vincent Eroukmanoff
Publication date
04-01-2023
Publisher
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Published in
The European Journal of Health Economics / Issue 9/2023
Print ISSN: 1618-7598
Electronic ISSN: 1618-7601
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10198-022-01560-9

Other articles of this Issue 9/2023

The European Journal of Health Economics 9/2023 Go to the issue