Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Neuropsychology Review 2/2017

01-06-2017 | Review

The Differential Contributions of Conceptual Representation Format and Language Structure to Levels of Semantic Abstraction Capacity

Author: Guido Gainotti

Published in: Neuropsychology Review | Issue 2/2017

Login to get access

Abstract

This paper reviews some controversies concerning the original and revised versions of the ‘hub-and-spoke’ model of conceptual representations and their implication for abstraction capacity levels. The ‘hub-and-spoke’ model, which is based on data gathered in patients with semantic dementia (SD), is the most authoritative model of conceptual knowledge. Patterson et al.’s (Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976-987, 2007) classical version of this model maintained that conceptual representations are stored in a unitary ‘amodal’ format in the right and left anterior temporal lobes (ATLs), because in SD the semantic disorder cuts across modalities and categories. Several authors questioned the unitary nature of these representations. They showed that the semantic impairment is ‘multi-modal’only in the advanced stages of SD, when atrophy affects the ATLs bilaterally, but that impariments can be modality-specific in lateralised (early) stages of the disease. In these cases, SD mainly affects lexical-semantic knowledge when atrophy predominates on the left side and pictorial representations when atrophy prevails on the right side. Some aspects of the model (i.e. the importance of spokes, the multimodal format of representations and the graded convergence of modalities within the ATLs), which had already been outlined by Rogers et al. (Psychological Review, 111(1), 205–235, 2004) in a computational model of SD, were strengthened by these results. The relevance of these theoretical problems and of empirical data concerning the neural substrate of concrete and abstract words is discussed critically. The conclusion of the review is that the highest levels of abstraction are due more to the structuring influence of language than to the format of representations.
Literature
go back to reference Acres, K., Taylor, K. I., Moss, H. E., Stamatakis, E. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2009). Complementary hemispheric asymmetries in object naming and recognition: a voxel-based correlational study. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 1836–1843.PubMedCrossRef Acres, K., Taylor, K. I., Moss, H. E., Stamatakis, E. A., & Tyler, L. K. (2009). Complementary hemispheric asymmetries in object naming and recognition: a voxel-based correlational study. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 1836–1843.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Antonucci, S. M., Beeson, P. M., Labiner, D. M., & Rapcsak, S. Z. (2008). Lexical retrieval and semantic knowledge in patients with left inferior temporal lobe lesions. Aphasiology, 22(3), 281–304.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Antonucci, S. M., Beeson, P. M., Labiner, D. M., & Rapcsak, S. Z. (2008). Lexical retrieval and semantic knowledge in patients with left inferior temporal lobe lesions. Aphasiology, 22(3), 281–304.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Arrigoni, G., & De Renzi, E. (1964). Constructional apraxia and hemispheric locus of lesion. Cortex, 1, 170–197.CrossRef Arrigoni, G., & De Renzi, E. (1964). Constructional apraxia and hemispheric locus of lesion. Cortex, 1, 170–197.CrossRef
go back to reference Bajada, C.J., Haroon, H.A., Azadbakht, H., Parker, G.J., Lambon Ralph, M.A., Cloutman, L.L. (2016). The tract terminations in the temporal lobe: Their location and associated functions. Cortex. 2016 Apr 1. Bajada, C.J., Haroon, H.A., Azadbakht, H., Parker, G.J., Lambon Ralph, M.A., Cloutman, L.L. (2016). The tract terminations in the temporal lobe: Their location and associated functions. Cortex. 2016 Apr 1.
go back to reference Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional imaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional imaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Binney, R. J., Embleton, K. V., Jefferies, E., Parker, G. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). The ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortion-corrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2728–2738.PubMedCrossRef Binney, R. J., Embleton, K. V., Jefferies, E., Parker, G. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). The ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortion-corrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2728–2738.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Binney, R. J., Parker, G. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). Convergent connectivity and graded specialization in the rostral human temporal lobe as revealed by diffusion-weighted imaging probabilistic tractography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1998–2014.PubMedCrossRef Binney, R. J., Parker, G. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). Convergent connectivity and graded specialization in the rostral human temporal lobe as revealed by diffusion-weighted imaging probabilistic tractography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1998–2014.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bloom, A. (1981). The linguistic shaping of thought: A study in the impact of language on thinking in China and the west. New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum Ass: Hillsdale. Bloom, A. (1981). The linguistic shaping of thought: A study in the impact of language on thinking in China and the west. New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum Ass: Hillsdale.
go back to reference Bonner, M. F., Vesely, L., Price, C., Anderson, C., Richmond, L., Farag, C., et al. (2009). Reversal of the concreteness effect in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 568–579.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bonner, M. F., Vesely, L., Price, C., Anderson, C., Richmond, L., Farag, C., et al. (2009). Reversal of the concreteness effect in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 568–579.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Borghi, A.M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: a study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, Feb 9;2:15. Borghi, A.M., Flumini, A., Cimatti, F., Marocco, D., Scorolli, C. (2011). Manipulating objects and telling words: a study on concrete and abstract words acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, Feb 9;2:15.
go back to reference Breedin, S. D., Saffran, E. M., & Coslett, H. B. (1994). Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 617–660.CrossRef Breedin, S. D., Saffran, E. M., & Coslett, H. B. (1994). Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 617–660.CrossRef
go back to reference Butler, C. R., Bramati, S. M., Miller, B. L., & Gorno-Tempini, M. L. (2009). The neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal semantic processing deficits in neurodegenerative disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 22, 73–80.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Butler, C. R., Bramati, S. M., Miller, B. L., & Gorno-Tempini, M. L. (2009). The neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal semantic processing deficits in neurodegenerative disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 22, 73–80.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 361–381.PubMedCrossRef Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 361–381.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caplan, D., Holmes, J. M., & Marshall, J. C. (1974). Word classes and hemispheric specialization. Neuropsychologia, 12, 331–337.PubMedCrossRef Caplan, D., Holmes, J. M., & Marshall, J. C. (1974). Word classes and hemispheric specialization. Neuropsychologia, 12, 331–337.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Caramazza, A., Hillis, A., Rapp, B. C., & Romani, C. (1990). The multiple semantic hypothesis: multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 161–189.CrossRef Caramazza, A., Hillis, A., Rapp, B. C., & Romani, C. (1990). The multiple semantic hypothesis: multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 161–189.CrossRef
go back to reference Catricalà, E., Della Rosa, P. A., Plebani, V., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). Abstract and concrete categories? Evidences from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia, 64, 271–281.PubMedCrossRef Catricalà, E., Della Rosa, P. A., Plebani, V., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2014). Abstract and concrete categories? Evidences from neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia, 64, 271–281.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cipolotti, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). Semantic memory and reading abilities: a case report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 104–110.PubMedCrossRef Cipolotti, L., & Warrington, E. K. (1995). Semantic memory and reading abilities: a case report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1, 104–110.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Coltheart, M. (1980). Deep dyslexia: A review of the syndrome. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 22–47). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Coltheart, M. (1980). Deep dyslexia: A review of the syndrome. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 22–47). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
go back to reference Corballis, M. C. (1993). The lopsided ape. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Corballis, M. C. (1993). The lopsided ape. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.PubMedCrossRef Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Damasio, A. R. (1990). Category-related recognition defects as a clue to the neural substrates of knowledge. Trends in Neurosciences, 13, 95–98.PubMedCrossRef Damasio, A. R. (1990). Category-related recognition defects as a clue to the neural substrates of knowledge. Trends in Neurosciences, 13, 95–98.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Day, J. (1977). Right-hemisphere language processing in normal right-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 518–528.PubMed Day, J. (1977). Right-hemisphere language processing in normal right-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 518–528.PubMed
go back to reference Degroot, A. M. B. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 824–845. Degroot, A. M. B. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 824–845.
go back to reference De Renzi, E. (1982). Disorders of space exploration and cognition. New York: Wiley and Sons. De Renzi, E. (1982). Disorders of space exploration and cognition. New York: Wiley and Sons.
go back to reference De Renzi, E., Scotti, G., & Spinnler, H. (1969). Perceptual and associative disorders of visual recognition. Their relation to the side of cerebral lesion. Neurology, 19, 634–642.PubMedCrossRef De Renzi, E., Scotti, G., & Spinnler, H. (1969). Perceptual and associative disorders of visual recognition. Their relation to the side of cerebral lesion. Neurology, 19, 634–642.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference De Renzi, E., & Spinnler, H. (1966). Visual recognition in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 142(6), 515–525.PubMedCrossRef De Renzi, E., & Spinnler, H. (1966). Visual recognition in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 142(6), 515–525.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Domoto-Reilly, K., Sapolsky, D., Brickhouse, M., & Dickerson, B. C. (2012). Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Naming impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with left anterior temporal lobe atrophy. NeuroImage, 63, 348–355.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Domoto-Reilly, K., Sapolsky, D., Brickhouse, M., & Dickerson, B. C. (2012). Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Naming impairment in Alzheimer’s disease is associated with left anterior temporal lobe atrophy. NeuroImage, 63, 348–355.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Dove, G. (2014). Thinking in words: language as an embodied medium of thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(3), 371–389.PubMedCrossRef Dove, G. (2014). Thinking in words: language as an embodied medium of thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(3), 371–389.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ellis, H. D., & Shepherd, J. W. (1974). Recognition of abstract and concrete words presented in left and right visual fields. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 1035–1036.PubMedCrossRef Ellis, H. D., & Shepherd, J. W. (1974). Recognition of abstract and concrete words presented in left and right visual fields. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 1035–1036.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Faglioni, P., Spinnler, H., & Vignolo, L. A. (1969). Contrasting behavior of right and left hemisphere-damaged patients on a discriminative and a semantic task of auditory recognition. Cortex, 5(4), 366–389.PubMedCrossRef Faglioni, P., Spinnler, H., & Vignolo, L. A. (1969). Contrasting behavior of right and left hemisphere-damaged patients on a discriminative and a semantic task of auditory recognition. Cortex, 5(4), 366–389.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fiebach, C. J., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Processing concrete words: fMRI evidence against a specific right-hemisphere involvement. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 62–70.PubMedCrossRef Fiebach, C. J., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Processing concrete words: fMRI evidence against a specific right-hemisphere involvement. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 62–70.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Fodor, J. (1987). Psychosemantics: The problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. Fodor, J. (1987). Psychosemantics: The problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Bevis, L., Davies, I. R., Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2008a). Categorical perception of color is lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105(9), 3221–3225.CrossRef Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Bevis, L., Davies, I. R., Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2008a). Categorical perception of color is lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105(9), 3221–3225.CrossRef
go back to reference Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Clifford, A., Kay, P., Regier, T., & Davies, I. R. (2008b). Lateralization of categorical perception of color changes with color term acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105(47), 18221–18225.CrossRef Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Clifford, A., Kay, P., Regier, T., & Davies, I. R. (2008b). Lateralization of categorical perception of color changes with color term acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105(47), 18221–18225.CrossRef
go back to reference Franklin, S. (1989). Dissociations in auditory word comprehension; evidence from nine fluent aphasic patients. Aphasiology, 3, 189–207.CrossRef Franklin, S. (1989). Dissociations in auditory word comprehension; evidence from nine fluent aphasic patients. Aphasiology, 3, 189–207.CrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G. (2007). Different patterns of famous people recognition disorders in patients with right and left anterior temporal lesions: a systematic review. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1591–1607.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G. (2007). Different patterns of famous people recognition disorders in patients with right and left anterior temporal lesions: a systematic review. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1591–1607.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G. (2011a). The organization and dissolution of semantic-conceptual knowledge: is the ‘amodal hub’ the only plausible model? Brain and Cognition, 75, 299–309.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G. (2011a). The organization and dissolution of semantic-conceptual knowledge: is the ‘amodal hub’ the only plausible model? Brain and Cognition, 75, 299–309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G. (2011b). What the study of voice recognition in normal subjects and brain-damaged patients tells us about models of familiar people recognition. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2273–2282.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G. (2011b). What the study of voice recognition in normal subjects and brain-damaged patients tells us about models of familiar people recognition. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2273–2282.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G. (2012). The format of conceptual representations disrupted in semantic dementia: a position paper. Cortex, 48, 521–529.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G. (2012). The format of conceptual representations disrupted in semantic dementia: a position paper. Cortex, 48, 521–529.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G. (2013). Laterality effects in normal subjects’ recognition of familiar faces, voices and names. Perceptual and representational components. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1151–1160.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G. (2013). Laterality effects in normal subjects’ recognition of familiar faces, voices and names. Perceptual and representational components. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1151–1160.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gainotti, G. (2015). Is the difference between right and left ATLs due to the distinction between general and social cognition or between verbal and non-verbal representations? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 296–312.PubMedCrossRef Gainotti, G. (2015). Is the difference between right and left ATLs due to the distinction between general and social cognition or between verbal and non-verbal representations? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 296–312.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Galton, C. J., Patterson, K., Graham, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Williams, G., Antoun, N., et al. (2001). Differing patterns of temporal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neurology, 57, 216–225.PubMedCrossRef Galton, C. J., Patterson, K., Graham, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Williams, G., Antoun, N., et al. (2001). Differing patterns of temporal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neurology, 57, 216–225.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain, 123(7), 1293–1326.PubMedCrossRef Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain, 123(7), 1293–1326.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
go back to reference Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Rankin, K. P., Woolley, J. D., Rosen, H. J., Phengrasamy, L., & Miller, B. L. (2004). Cognitive and behavioral profile in a case of right anterior temporal lobe neurodegeneration. Cortex, 40, 631–644.PubMedCrossRef Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Rankin, K. P., Woolley, J. D., Rosen, H. J., Phengrasamy, L., & Miller, B. L. (2004). Cognitive and behavioral profile in a case of right anterior temporal lobe neurodegeneration. Cortex, 40, 631–644.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Graham, K. S., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1995). Progressive pure anomia: insufficient activation of phonology by meaning. Neurocase, 1, 25–38.CrossRef Graham, K. S., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1995). Progressive pure anomia: insufficient activation of phonology by meaning. Neurocase, 1, 25–38.CrossRef
go back to reference Grossman, M., & Wilson, M. (1987). Stimulus categorization by brain-damaged patients. Brain and Cognition, 6(1), 55–71.PubMedCrossRef Grossman, M., & Wilson, M. (1987). Stimulus categorization by brain-damaged patients. Brain and Cognition, 6(1), 55–71.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behaviour: A neurophysiological theory. Hoboken: Wiley. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behaviour: A neurophysiological theory. Hoboken: Wiley.
go back to reference Hines, D. (1976). Recognition of verbs, abstract nouns and concrete nouns from the left and right visual half-fields. Neuropsychologia, 14, 211–216.PubMedCrossRef Hines, D. (1976). Recognition of verbs, abstract nouns and concrete nouns from the left and right visual half-fields. Neuropsychologia, 14, 211–216.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hines, D. (1977). Differences in tachistoscopic recognition between abstract and concrete words as a function of visual half-field and frequency. Cortex, 13, 66–73.PubMedCrossRef Hines, D. (1977). Differences in tachistoscopic recognition between abstract and concrete words as a function of visual half-field and frequency. Cortex, 13, 66–73.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain, 115, 1783–1806.PubMedCrossRef Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain, 115, 1783–1806.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Binney, R. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge. Cortex, 63, 250–266.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hoffman, P., Binney, R. J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). Differing contributions of inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal cortex to concrete and abstract conceptual knowledge. Cortex, 63, 250–266.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex plays an executive regulation role in comprehension of abstract words: convergent neuropsychological and rTMS evidence. Journal of Neuroscience, 46, 15450–15456.CrossRef Hoffman, P., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex plays an executive regulation role in comprehension of abstract words: convergent neuropsychological and rTMS evidence. Journal of Neuroscience, 46, 15450–15456.CrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). The degraded concept representation system in semantic dementia: damage to pan-modal hub, then visual spoke. Brain, 135, 3770–3780.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). The degraded concept representation system in semantic dementia: damage to pan-modal hub, then visual spoke. Brain, 135, 3770–3780.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2013). Be concrete to be comprehended: consistent imageability effects in semantic dementia for nouns, verbs, synonyms and associates. Cortex, 49(5), 1206–1218.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2013). Be concrete to be comprehended: consistent imageability effects in semantic dementia for nouns, verbs, synonyms and associates. Cortex, 49(5), 1206–1218.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Reverse concreteness effects are not a typical feature of semantic dementia: evidence for the hub-and-spoke model of conceptual representation. Cerebral Cortex, 21(9), 2103–2112.PubMedCrossRef Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Reverse concreteness effects are not a typical feature of semantic dementia: evidence for the hub-and-spoke model of conceptual representation. Cerebral Cortex, 21(9), 2103–2112.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1992). Pyramids and palm trees: Access from pictures and words. Thames Valley Test Company: Bury St Edmunds (UK). Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1992). Pyramids and palm trees: Access from pictures and words. Thames Valley Test Company: Bury St Edmunds (UK).
go back to reference Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1988). On the case for multiple semantic systems - a reply. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 143–150.CrossRef Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1988). On the case for multiple semantic systems - a reply. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 143–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Humphries, C., Willard, K., Buchsbaum, B., & Hickok, G. (2001). Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12, 1749–1752.PubMedCrossRef Humphries, C., Willard, K., Buchsbaum, B., & Hickok, G. (2001). Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12, 1749–1752.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hurley, R. S., Paller, K. A., Rogalski, E. J., & Mesulam, M. M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of object naming and word comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(14), 4848–4855.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hurley, R. S., Paller, K. A., Rogalski, E. J., & Mesulam, M. M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of object naming and word comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(14), 4848–4855.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Ikeda, M., Patterson, K., Graham, K. S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). A horse of a different colour: do patients with semantic dementia recognise different versions of the same object as the same? Neuropsychologia, 44, 566–575.PubMedCrossRef Ikeda, M., Patterson, K., Graham, K. S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). A horse of a different colour: do patients with semantic dementia recognise different versions of the same object as the same? Neuropsychologia, 44, 566–575.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, R. L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). The nature and neural correlates of semantic association versus conceptual similarity. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4319–4333.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jackson, R. L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015). The nature and neural correlates of semantic association versus conceptual similarity. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4319–4333.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 130–136. James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 130–136.
go back to reference Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 23, 492–499.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., Jones, R. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2009). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 23, 492–499.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Joseph, R. (1988). The right cerebral hemisphere: emotion, music, visual-spatial skills, body-image, dreams, and awareness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(5), 630–673.PubMedCrossRef Joseph, R. (1988). The right cerebral hemisphere: emotion, music, visual-spatial skills, body-image, dreams, and awareness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(5), 630–673.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Katz, R. B., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Deep dysphasia: analysis of a rare form of repetition disorder. Brain and Language, 39, 153–185.PubMedCrossRef Katz, R. B., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Deep dysphasia: analysis of a rare form of repetition disorder. Brain and Language, 39, 153–185.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Knott, R., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Lexical and semantic binding effects in short-term memory: evidence from semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 1165–1216.CrossRef Knott, R., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Lexical and semantic binding effects in short-term memory: evidence from semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 1165–1216.CrossRef
go back to reference Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2014). Neurocognitive insights on conceptual knowledge and its breakdown. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B Biological Sciences, 369, 1–12. Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2014). Neurocognitive insights on conceptual knowledge and its breakdown. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B Biological Sciences, 369, 1–12.
go back to reference Lambon Ralph, M. A., McClelland, J. L., Patterson, K., Galton, C. J., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). No right to speak? The relationship between object naming and semantic impairment: neuropsychological evidence and a computational model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 341–356.PubMedCrossRef Lambon Ralph, M. A., McClelland, J. L., Patterson, K., Galton, C. J., & Hodges, J. R. (2001). No right to speak? The relationship between object naming and semantic impairment: neuropsychological evidence and a computational model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 341–356.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Patterson, K. (2008). Generalization and differentiation in semantic memory insights from semantic dementia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 61–76.PubMedCrossRef Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Patterson, K. (2008). Generalization and differentiation in semantic memory insights from semantic dementia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 61–76.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lambon Ralph, M. A., Pobric, G., & Jefferies, E. (2009). Conceptual knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: convergent evidence from rTMS. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 832–838.PubMedCrossRef Lambon Ralph, M. A., Pobric, G., & Jefferies, E. (2009). Conceptual knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: convergent evidence from rTMS. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 832–838.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lambon Ralph, M. A., Sage, K., Jones, R. W., & Mayberry, E. J. (2010). Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal lobes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(6), 2717–2722.CrossRef Lambon Ralph, M. A., Sage, K., Jones, R. W., & Mayberry, E. J. (2010). Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal lobes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(6), 2717–2722.CrossRef
go back to reference Libon, D. J., Rascovsky, K., Powers, J., Irwin, D. J., Boller, A., Weinberg, D., et al. (2013). Comparative semantic profiles in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 136(8), 2497–2509.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Libon, D. J., Rascovsky, K., Powers, J., Irwin, D. J., Boller, A., Weinberg, D., et al. (2013). Comparative semantic profiles in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 136(8), 2497–2509.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Loiselle, M., Rouleau, I., Nguyen, D. K., Dubeau, F., Macoir, J., Whatmough, C., et al. (2012). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in patients with selective anterior temporal lobe resection and in patients with selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 630–639.PubMedCrossRef Loiselle, M., Rouleau, I., Nguyen, D. K., Dubeau, F., Macoir, J., Whatmough, C., et al. (2012). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in patients with selective anterior temporal lobe resection and in patients with selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 630–639.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lyons, J. (1981). Language and linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lyons, J. (1981). Language and linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Macoir, J. (2009). Is a plum a memory problem? Longitudinal study of the reversal of concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 518–535.PubMedCrossRef Macoir, J. (2009). Is a plum a memory problem? Longitudinal study of the reversal of concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 518–535.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McCarthy, R.A., & Warrington, E.K. (2016). Past, present, and prospects: reflections 40 years on from the selective impairment of semantic memory (Warrington, 1975). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 1941–1968. McCarthy, R.A., & Warrington, E.K. (2016). Past, present, and prospects: reflections 40 years on from the selective impairment of semantic memory (Warrington, 1975). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 1941–1968.
go back to reference Merck, C., Jonin, P. Y., Laisney, M., Vichard, H., & Belliard, S. (2014). When the zebra loses its stripes but is still in the savannah: results from a semantic priming paradigm in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 53, 221–232.PubMedCrossRef Merck, C., Jonin, P. Y., Laisney, M., Vichard, H., & Belliard, S. (2014). When the zebra loses its stripes but is still in the savannah: results from a semantic priming paradigm in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 53, 221–232.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mesulam, M. M., Wieneke, C., Hurley, R., Rademaker, A., Thompson, C. K., Weintraub, S., et al. (2013). Words and objects at the tip of the left temporal lobe in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 136(2), 601–618.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Mesulam, M. M., Wieneke, C., Hurley, R., Rademaker, A., Thompson, C. K., Weintraub, S., et al. (2013). Words and objects at the tip of the left temporal lobe in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 136(2), 601–618.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Mion, M., Patterson, K., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Pengas, G., Izquierdo-Garcia, D., Hong, Y. T., et al. (2010). What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain, 133(11), 3256–3268.PubMedCrossRef Mion, M., Patterson, K., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Pengas, G., Izquierdo-Garcia, D., Hong, Y. T., et al. (2010). What the left and right anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain, 133(11), 3256–3268.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
go back to reference Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford. England: Oxford University Press. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford. England: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Papagno, C., Capasso, R., & Miceli, G. (2009). Reversed concreteness effect for nouns in a subject with semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1138–1148.PubMedCrossRef Papagno, C., Capasso, R., & Miceli, G. (2009). Reversed concreteness effect for nouns in a subject with semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1138–1148.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976–987.PubMedCrossRef Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976–987.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Phylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells to the mind’s brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.CrossRef Phylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells to the mind’s brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.CrossRef
go back to reference Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2007). Anterior temporal lobes mediate semantic representation: mimicking semantic dementia by using rTMS in normal participants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 20137–20141. Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2007). Anterior temporal lobes mediate semantic representation: mimicking semantic dementia by using rTMS in normal participants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 20137–20141.
go back to reference Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010a). Amodal semantic representations depend on both anterior temporal lobes: evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1336–1342.PubMedCrossRef Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010a). Amodal semantic representations depend on both anterior temporal lobes: evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1336–1342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010b). Category-specific versus category-general semantic impairment induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Current Biology, 20(10), 964–968.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010b). Category-specific versus category-general semantic impairment induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Current Biology, 20(10), 964–968.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Pobric, G., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Jefferies, E. (2009). The role of the anterior temporal lobes in the comprehension of concrete and abstract words: rTMS evidence. Cortex, 45, 1104–1110.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pobric, G., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Jefferies, E. (2009). The role of the anterior temporal lobes in the comprehension of concrete and abstract words: rTMS evidence. Cortex, 45, 1104–1110.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Reilly, J., Peelle, J. E., Garcia, A., & Crutch, S. J. (2016). Linking somatic and symbolic representation in semantic memory: the dynamic multilevel reactivation framework. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(4), 1002–1014.PubMedCrossRef Reilly, J., Peelle, J. E., Garcia, A., & Crutch, S. J. (2016). Linking somatic and symbolic representation in semantic memory: the dynamic multilevel reactivation framework. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(4), 1002–1014.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rice, G. E., Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015a). Graded specialization within and between the anterior temporal lobes. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1359, 84–97.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rice, G. E., Hoffman, P., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2015a). Graded specialization within and between the anterior temporal lobes. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1359, 84–97.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Rice, G. E., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Hoffman, P. (2015b). The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowledge: an ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4374–4391.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rice, G. E., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Hoffman, P. (2015b). The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowledge: an ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 25(11), 4374–4391.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., McClelland, J. L., Hodges, J. R., et al. (2004). The structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. Psychological Review, 111(1), 205–235.PubMedCrossRef Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., McClelland, J. L., Hodges, J. R., et al. (2004). The structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. Psychological Review, 111(1), 205–235.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Schapiro, A. C., McClelland, J. L., Welbourne, S. R., Rogers, T. T., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2013). Why bilateral damage is worse than unilateral damage to the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(12), 2107–2123.PubMedCrossRef Schapiro, A. C., McClelland, J. L., Welbourne, S. R., Rogers, T. T., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2013). Why bilateral damage is worse than unilateral damage to the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(12), 2107–2123.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shimotake, A., Matsumoto, R., Ueno, T., Kunieda, T., Saito, S., Hoffman, P., et al. (2015). Direct exploration of the role of the ventral anterior temporal lobe in semantic memory: cortical stimulation and local field potential evidence from subdural grid electrodes. Cerebral Cortex, 25(10), 3802–3817.PubMedCrossRef Shimotake, A., Matsumoto, R., Ueno, T., Kunieda, T., Saito, S., Hoffman, P., et al. (2015). Direct exploration of the role of the ventral anterior temporal lobe in semantic memory: cortical stimulation and local field potential evidence from subdural grid electrodes. Cerebral Cortex, 25(10), 3802–3817.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Skipper-Kallal, L. M., Mirman, D., & Olson, I. R. (2015). Converging evidence from fMRI and aphasia that the left temporoparietal cortex has an essential role in representing abstract semantic knowledge. Cortex, 69, 104–120.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Skipper-Kallal, L. M., Mirman, D., & Olson, I. R. (2015). Converging evidence from fMRI and aphasia that the left temporoparietal cortex has an essential role in representing abstract semantic knowledge. Cortex, 69, 104–120.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Snowden, J., Goulding, P. J., & Neary, D. (1989). Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behavioural Neurology, 2, 167–182. Snowden, J., Goulding, P. J., & Neary, D. (1989). Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behavioural Neurology, 2, 167–182.
go back to reference Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., & Neary, D. (2004). Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia. Brain, 127(4), 860–872.PubMedCrossRef Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., & Neary, D. (2004). Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia. Brain, 127(4), 860–872.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thierry, G., Giraud, A. L., & Price, C. (2003). Hemispheric dissociation in access to the human semantic system. Neuron, 38(3), 499–506.PubMedCrossRef Thierry, G., Giraud, A. L., & Price, C. (2003). Hemispheric dissociation in access to the human semantic system. Neuron, 38(3), 499–506.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thierry, G., & Price, C. (2006). Dissociating verbal and nonverbal conceptual processing in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 1018–1028.PubMedCrossRef Thierry, G., & Price, C. (2006). Dissociating verbal and nonverbal conceptual processing in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 1018–1028.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Vignolo, L. A. (1982). Auditory agnosia. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences Series B, 298(1089), 49–57.CrossRef Vignolo, L. A. (1982). Auditory agnosia. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Biological Sciences Series B, 298(1089), 49–57.CrossRef
go back to reference Visser, M., Jefferies, E., Embleton, K. V., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). Both the middle temporal gyrus and the ventral anterior temporal area are crucial for multimodal semantic processing: distortion-corrected fMRI evidence for a double gradient of information convergence in the temporal lobes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 1766–1778.PubMedCrossRef Visser, M., Jefferies, E., Embleton, K. V., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012). Both the middle temporal gyrus and the ventral anterior temporal area are crucial for multimodal semantic processing: distortion-corrected fMRI evidence for a double gradient of information convergence in the temporal lobes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 1766–1778.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1459–1468.PubMedCrossRef Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1459–1468.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMedCrossRef Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category-specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106, 859–878.PubMedCrossRef Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1983). Category-specific access dysphasia. Brain, 106, 859–878.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1987). Categories of knowledge: further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1465–1473.CrossRef Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. (1987). Categories of knowledge: further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1465–1473.CrossRef
go back to reference Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category-specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.PubMedCrossRef Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category-specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yi, H. A., Moore, P., & Grossman, M. (2007). Reversal of the concreteness effect for verbs in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 21, 9–19.PubMedCrossRef Yi, H. A., Moore, P., & Grossman, M. (2007). Reversal of the concreteness effect for verbs in semantic dementia. Neuropsychology, 21, 9–19.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zannino, G. D., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2015). The contribution of neurodegenerative diseases to the modelling of semantic memory: a new proposal and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia, 75, 274–290.PubMedCrossRef Zannino, G. D., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2015). The contribution of neurodegenerative diseases to the modelling of semantic memory: a new proposal and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia, 75, 274–290.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zannino, G. D., Perri, R., Monaco, M., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2014). The special status of verbal knowledge in semantic memory: evidence from performance of semantically impaired subjects on verbalizable and non-verbalizable versions of the object decision task. Brain and Language, 128, 9–17.PubMedCrossRef Zannino, G. D., Perri, R., Monaco, M., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2014). The special status of verbal knowledge in semantic memory: evidence from performance of semantically impaired subjects on verbalizable and non-verbalizable versions of the object decision task. Brain and Language, 128, 9–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zipse, L., Norton, A., Marchina, S., & Schlaug, G. (2012). When right is all that is left: plasticity of right-hemisphere tracts in a young aphasic patient. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 237–245.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Zipse, L., Norton, A., Marchina, S., & Schlaug, G. (2012). When right is all that is left: plasticity of right-hemisphere tracts in a young aphasic patient. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 237–245.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Metadata
Title
The Differential Contributions of Conceptual Representation Format and Language Structure to Levels of Semantic Abstraction Capacity
Author
Guido Gainotti
Publication date
01-06-2017
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Neuropsychology Review / Issue 2/2017
Print ISSN: 1040-7308
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6660
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9339-8

Other articles of this Issue 2/2017

Neuropsychology Review 2/2017 Go to the issue