Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 6/2014

01-06-2014 | Research Article

The contribution of cognitive, kinematic, and dynamic factors to anticipatory grasp selection

Authors: Oliver Herbort, Martin V. Butz, Wilfried Kunde

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 6/2014

Login to get access

Abstract

Object-directed grasping movements are usually adjusted in anticipation of the direction and extent of a subsequent object rotation. Such anticipatory grasp selections have been mostly explained in terms of the kinematics of the arm movement. However, object rotations of different directions and extents also differ in their dynamics and in how the tasks are represented. Here, we examined how the dynamics, the kinematics, and the cognitive representation of an object manipulation affect anticipatory grasp selections. We asked participants to grasp an object and rotate it by different angles and in different directions. To examine the influence of dynamic factors, we varied the object’s weight. To examine the influence of the cognitive task representation, we instructed identical object rotations as either toward-top or away-from-top rotations. While instructed object rotation and cognitive task representation did affect grasp selection over the entire course of the experiment, a rather small effect of object weight only appeared late in the experiment. We suggest that grasp selections are determined on different levels. The representation of the kinematics of the object movement determines grasp selection on a trial-by-trial basis. The effect of object weight affects grasp selection by a slower adaptation process. This result implies that even simple motor acts, such as grasping, can only be understood when cognitive factors, such as the task representation, are taken into account.
Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
With grasp selection, we refer to the orientation of the hand and forearm when grasping the object.
 
2
If participants were within 3° of the target orientation the message “Ausgezeichnet!!!” (German for “Excellent!!!”) appeared. Within 3°–7.5°, the message “Sehr gut!” (“Very good!”) appeared. Within 7.5°–15°, the text “Gut, aber zu weit links/rechts.” (“Good, but too far to the left/right”) appeared, depending on the direction of the error. If errors exceeded 15°, the text “Fehler” (“error”) was shown. If participants were too slow or did not lift the cylinder, the text “Schneller oder Rad höher anheben” (“Faster or lift cylinder higher”) appeared.
 
3
This is supported by the data in two ways. First, at the time of grasping, the yaw (left–right) and pitch (up–down) angle of the forearm with respect to an external coordinate system revealed little variability over the trials of each participant (mean SDyaw = 7.3°, mean SDpitch = 6.6°, for comparison, the mean of the participants’ SD of FOGRASP was 66.4°). Second, on average, the difference between the highest and lowest yaw and pitch angle recorded during each cylinder rotation was 8.9° and 13.9°, respectively. The low variability of the forearms position shows that the forearm was stretched or only slightly flexed when grasping and moving the object, suggesting that FOGRASP reflects mostly pronation and supination of the forearm.
 
4
Greenhouse–Geisser corrected p values but uncorrected dfs are reported.
 
5
Descriptively, the cylinder presented in the first part tended to be grasped more supine, with the exception of away-from-top trials in blocks 3 + 4, resulting in a marginally significant interactions between weight, group, block (and stimulus type), p = 0.069 (and p = 0.063, respectively). No other effect approached significance, all ps ≥ 0.206.
 
6
Δstimulus = 0.5 × [(FOCCW,TOWARD-TOP − FOCCW,AWAY-FROM-TOP) + (FOCW,AWAY-FROM-TOP − FOCW,TOWARD-TOP)]; Δweight = 0.5 × [(FOCCW,HEAVY − FOCCW,LIGHT) + (FOCW,LIGHT − FOCW,HEAVY)]; where FO X,Y denotes FOGRASP averaged over all other factors than implied by X and Y. For examples, FOCCW,TOWARD-TOP refers to the FOGRASP averaged over all counterclockwise target angles and both weights.
 
7
MTROTs tended to decrease from the earlier blocks to the later blocks (p = 0.068). The interaction between rotation angle and stimulus type approached significance (p = 0.070). This interaction was neither based on a consistent effect of the rotation direction (p = 0.210) nor amplitude (p = 0.438). The three-way interaction between weight, group, and stimulus trials was modulated marginally by block (p = 0.080). All other effects did not approach significance (all ps ≥ 0.173).
 
8
The effect of block approached significance (p = 0.72, all other ps ≥ 0.089).
 
9
As our participants were mostly female, these values are likely to underestimate the exerted torques in the task respective the maximum torques our participants would be able to produce.
 
10
An inspection of single-trial data revealed that the absence of the effect of stimulus type on FOGRASP cannot be explained by the effect being present in a subset of trials and being subsequently averaged out in the aggregated data.
 
Literature
go back to reference Bock O, Steinberg F (2012) Age-related deficits of manual grasping in a laboratory versus in an everyday-like setting. Ageing Res 4(e7):48–52. doi:10.4081/ar.2012.e7 Bock O, Steinberg F (2012) Age-related deficits of manual grasping in a laboratory versus in an everyday-like setting. Ageing Res 4(e7):48–52. doi:10.​4081/​ar.​2012.​e7
go back to reference Coren S (1993) The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: norms for young adults. Bull Psychon Soc 31(1):1–3CrossRef Coren S (1993) The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: norms for young adults. Bull Psychon Soc 31(1):1–3CrossRef
go back to reference Eder AB, Rothermund K (2008) When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen 137:262–281PubMedCrossRef Eder AB, Rothermund K (2008) When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen 137:262–281PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Krakauer JW, Ghilardi M-F, Ghez C (1999) Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nat Neurosci 2(11):1026–1031. doi:10.1038/14826 PubMedCrossRef Krakauer JW, Ghilardi M-F, Ghez C (1999) Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nat Neurosci 2(11):1026–1031. doi:10.​1038/​14826 PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kunde W, Weigelt M (2005) Goal congruency in bimanual object manipulation. J Exp Psychol 31(1):145–156 Kunde W, Weigelt M (2005) Goal congruency in bimanual object manipulation. J Exp Psychol 31(1):145–156
go back to reference Neumann R, Lozo L, Kunde W (2014) Not all behaviors are controlled in the same way: Different mechanisms underlie manual and facial approach and avoidance responses. J Exp Psychol Gen 143(1):1–8. doi:10.1037/a0032259 Neumann R, Lozo L, Kunde W (2014) Not all behaviors are controlled in the same way: Different mechanisms underlie manual and facial approach and avoidance responses. J Exp Psychol Gen 143(1):1–8. doi:10.​1037/​a0032259
go back to reference Rosenbaum DA, Marchak F, Barnes HJ, Vaughan J, Slotta JD, Jorgensen MJ (1990) Constraints for action selection: overhand versus underhand grips. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance, vol XIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 321–345 Rosenbaum DA, Marchak F, Barnes HJ, Vaughan J, Slotta JD, Jorgensen MJ (1990) Constraints for action selection: overhand versus underhand grips. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance, vol XIII. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 321–345
go back to reference Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J, Barnes HJ, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. J Exp Psychol 18(5):1058–1073 Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J, Barnes HJ, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. J Exp Psychol 18(5):1058–1073
go back to reference Rosenbaum DA, van Heugten CM, Caldwell GE (1996) From cognition to biomechanics and back: the end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychol 94:59–85CrossRef Rosenbaum DA, van Heugten CM, Caldwell GE (1996) From cognition to biomechanics and back: the end-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychol 94:59–85CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (2001) Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. Psychol Rev 108(4):709–734PubMedCrossRef Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (2001) Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. Psychol Rev 108(4):709–734PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5(11):1226–1235PubMedCrossRef Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci 5(11):1226–1235PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wing AM, Flanagan JR, Richardson J (1997) Anticipatory postural adjustments in stance and grip. Exp Brain Res 116(1):122–130PubMedCrossRef Wing AM, Flanagan JR, Richardson J (1997) Anticipatory postural adjustments in stance and grip. Exp Brain Res 116(1):122–130PubMedCrossRef
Metadata
Title
The contribution of cognitive, kinematic, and dynamic factors to anticipatory grasp selection
Authors
Oliver Herbort
Martin V. Butz
Wilfried Kunde
Publication date
01-06-2014
Publisher
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 6/2014
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-3849-5

Other articles of this Issue 6/2014

Experimental Brain Research 6/2014 Go to the issue