Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 3-4/2011

Open Access 01-06-2011 | Research Article

Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults

Authors: Jennifer Cook, Geoffrey Bird

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 3-4/2011

Login to get access

Abstract

Previous studies have demonstrated a bidirectional relationship between social attitudes and imitation in adults: pro-social attitudes promote imitation, and imitation further increases positive social attitudes. Social attitudes and the social brain are developing throughout the adolescent years. Thus, the aim of this study was to test whether pro-social attitudes promote imitation in an Adolescent Group to the same extent as in an Adult Group. Participants were primed with pro-social or non-social words in a Scrambled Sentence Priming task. They then completed an Imitation task wherein participants were required to perform a lift action with either the index or middle finger, whilst observing either a compatible action (e.g. index finger response and observed index finger lift) or an incompatible action (e.g. index finger response and observed middle finger lift). In an Effector Priming control condition, observed fingers remained stationary but a semi-transparent green mask was added to either the compatible or incompatible finger. The magnitude of the Imitation Effect and Effector Priming Effect was calculated by subtracting reaction times on compatible trials from those on incompatible trials. In the Adult Group, social priming specifically modulated the Imitation Effect: pro-social priming produced a larger Imitation Effect but did not modulate the Effector Priming Effect. In adolescents, however, no effect of social priming was seen on either the Imitation or Effector Priming measures. We consider possible explanations for these results including the immature development of social brain regions and reduced experience of the relationship between social attitudes and imitation in adolescence.
Literature
go back to reference Bach P, Tipper SP (2007) Implicit action encoding influences personal-trait judgments. Cognition 102:151–178PubMedCrossRef Bach P, Tipper SP (2007) Implicit action encoding influences personal-trait judgments. Cognition 102:151–178PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bailenson JN, Yee N (2005) Digital chameleons: automatic assimilation of nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual environments. Psychol Sci 16(10):814–819PubMedCrossRef Bailenson JN, Yee N (2005) Digital chameleons: automatic assimilation of nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual environments. Psychol Sci 16(10):814–819PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berger SM, Hadley SW (1975) Some effects of a model’s performance on an observer’s electromyographic activity. Am J of Psychol 88:263–276CrossRef Berger SM, Hadley SW (1975) Some effects of a model’s performance on an observer’s electromyographic activity. Am J of Psychol 88:263–276CrossRef
go back to reference Bernieri FJ (1988) Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher–student interactions. J Nonverbal Behav 12:120–138CrossRef Bernieri FJ (1988) Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher–student interactions. J Nonverbal Behav 12:120–138CrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore SJ (2010) The developing social brain: implications for education. Neuron 65(6):744–747PubMedCrossRef Blakemore SJ (2010) The developing social brain: implications for education. Neuron 65(6):744–747PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blakemore S, den Ouden H, Choudhury S, Frith C (2007) Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Soc Cog Affect Neurosci 2(2):130–139CrossRef Blakemore S, den Ouden H, Choudhury S, Frith C (2007) Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions. Soc Cog Affect Neurosci 2(2):130–139CrossRef
go back to reference Brass M, Heyes CM (2005) Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends Cogn Sci 9:489–495PubMedCrossRef Brass M, Heyes CM (2005) Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends Cogn Sci 9:489–495PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass M, Bekkering H, Wohlschlager A, Prinz W (2000) Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain Cogn 44:124–143PubMedCrossRef Brass M, Bekkering H, Wohlschlager A, Prinz W (2000) Compatibility between observed and executed finger movements: comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain Cogn 44:124–143PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass M, Derrfuss J, Matthes-von Cramon G, von Cramon DY (2003) Imitative response tendencies in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology 17(2):265–271PubMedCrossRef Brass M, Derrfuss J, Matthes-von Cramon G, von Cramon DY (2003) Imitative response tendencies in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology 17(2):265–271PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brass M, Derrfuss J, von Cramon DY (2005) The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation. Neuropsychologia 43:89–98PubMedCrossRef Brass M, Derrfuss J, von Cramon DY (2005) The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation. Neuropsychologia 43:89–98PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Brothers L (1990) The social brain: a project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neurosci 1:27–51 Brothers L (1990) The social brain: a project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neurosci 1:27–51
go back to reference Brown BB (2004) Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L (eds) Handbook of adolescent psychology, 2nd edn. Wiley, Hoboken, NJ Brown BB (2004) Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L (eds) Handbook of adolescent psychology, 2nd edn. Wiley, Hoboken, NJ
go back to reference Burnett S, Bird G, Moll J, Frith C, Blakemore S (2009) Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. J Cogn Neurosci 21(9):1736–1750PubMedCrossRef Burnett S, Bird G, Moll J, Frith C, Blakemore S (2009) Development during adolescence of the neural processing of social emotion. J Cogn Neurosci 21(9):1736–1750PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Catmur C, Heyes CM (2010) Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. doi:10.1037/a0019325 Catmur C, Heyes CM (2010) Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. doi:10.​1037/​a0019325
go back to reference Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(6):893–910PubMedCrossRef Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(6):893–910PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chartrand TL, Jeffers V (2003) Consequences of automatic goal pursuit and the case of nonconscious mimicry. In: Forgas JP, Williams KD, von Hippel W (eds) Responding to the social world: implicit and explicit processes in social judgments and decisions. Psychology Press, Philadelphia Chartrand TL, Jeffers V (2003) Consequences of automatic goal pursuit and the case of nonconscious mimicry. In: Forgas JP, Williams KD, von Hippel W (eds) Responding to the social world: implicit and explicit processes in social judgments and decisions. Psychology Press, Philadelphia
go back to reference Cheng CM, Chartrand TL (2003) Self-monitoring without awareness: using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. J Pers Soc Psychol 85(6):1170–1179PubMedCrossRef Cheng CM, Chartrand TL (2003) Self-monitoring without awareness: using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. J Pers Soc Psychol 85(6):1170–1179PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Frith U, Frith C (2010) The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365(1537):165–176PubMedCrossRef Frith U, Frith C (2010) The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365(1537):165–176PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Galvan A (2010) Neural plasticity of development and learning. Hum Brain Mapp 31(6):879–890PubMed Galvan A (2010) Neural plasticity of development and learning. Hum Brain Mapp 31(6):879–890PubMed
go back to reference Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL (1999) Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal mri study. Nat Neurosci 2(10):861–863PubMedCrossRef Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL (1999) Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal mri study. Nat Neurosci 2(10):861–863PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gillmeister H, Catmur C, Liepelt R, Brass M, Heyes CM (2008) Experience-based priming of body parts: a study of action imitation. Brain Res 1217:157–170PubMedCrossRef Gillmeister H, Catmur C, Liepelt R, Brass M, Heyes CM (2008) Experience-based priming of body parts: a study of action imitation. Brain Res 1217:157–170PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Harter S (1990) Developmental differences in the nature of self-representations: implications for the understanding, assessment, and treatment of maladaptive behaviour. Cogn Therapy Res 14(2):113–142CrossRef Harter S (1990) Developmental differences in the nature of self-representations: implications for the understanding, assessment, and treatment of maladaptive behaviour. Cogn Therapy Res 14(2):113–142CrossRef
go back to reference Heyes CM, Ray ED (2000) What is the significance of imitation in animals? Advances in the Study of Behavior 29:215–245CrossRef Heyes CM, Ray ED (2000) What is the significance of imitation in animals? Advances in the Study of Behavior 29:215–245CrossRef
go back to reference Heyes CM, Bird G, Johnson H, Haggard P (2005) Experience modulates automatic imitation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 22(2):233–240PubMedCrossRef Heyes CM, Bird G, Johnson H, Haggard P (2005) Experience modulates automatic imitation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 22(2):233–240PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huttenlocher PR (1979) Synaptic density in human frontal cortex—developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res 163(2):195–205PubMedCrossRef Huttenlocher PR (1979) Synaptic density in human frontal cortex—developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res 163(2):195–205PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kühn S, Müller BCN, van Baaren RB, Wietzker A, Dijksterhuis A, Brass M (2010) Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated. Soc Neurosci 5(4):384–392PubMedCrossRef Kühn S, Müller BCN, van Baaren RB, Wietzker A, Dijksterhuis A, Brass M (2010) Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated. Soc Neurosci 5(4):384–392PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lafrance M (1979) Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Soc Psychol Quart 42:66–70CrossRef Lafrance M (1979) Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Soc Psychol Quart 42:66–70CrossRef
go back to reference LaFrance M (1982) Posture mirroring and rapport. In: Davis M (ed) Interaction rhythms: periodicity in communicative behaviour. Human Sciences Press, New York LaFrance M (1982) Posture mirroring and rapport. In: Davis M (ed) Interaction rhythms: periodicity in communicative behaviour. Human Sciences Press, New York
go back to reference Lafrance M, Broadbent M (1976) Group rapport: posture sharing as a nonverbal indicator. Group Organization Studies 1:328–333CrossRef Lafrance M, Broadbent M (1976) Group rapport: posture sharing as a nonverbal indicator. Group Organization Studies 1:328–333CrossRef
go back to reference Lakin JL, Chartrand TL (2003) Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci 14(4):334–339PubMedCrossRef Lakin JL, Chartrand TL (2003) Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci 14(4):334–339PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liepelt R, Brass M (2010) Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Exp Psychol 57(3):221–227PubMed Liepelt R, Brass M (2010) Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Exp Psychol 57(3):221–227PubMed
go back to reference Paus T (2005) Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 9(2):60–68PubMedCrossRef Paus T (2005) Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 9(2):60–68PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88(3):B11–B21PubMedCrossRef Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88(3):B11–B21PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):70–76PubMedCrossRef Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):70–76PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebastian C, Burnett S, Blakemore SJ (2008) Development of the self-concept during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 12(11):441–446PubMedCrossRef Sebastian C, Burnett S, Blakemore SJ (2008) Development of the self-concept during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci 12(11):441–446PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sebastian C, Viding E, Williams KD, Blakemore S (2010) Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain Cog 72(1):134–145CrossRef Sebastian C, Viding E, Williams KD, Blakemore S (2010) Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain Cog 72(1):134–145CrossRef
go back to reference Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Wise SP (2008) Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 28(14):3586–3594PubMedCrossRef Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Wise SP (2008) Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 28(14):3586–3594PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Batth R, Jernigan TL, Toga AW (1999) Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage 9(6 Pt 1):587–597 Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Batth R, Jernigan TL, Toga AW (1999) Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage 9(6 Pt 1):587–597
go back to reference Spengler S, von Cramon DY, Brass M (2010a) Resisting motor mimicry: control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions. Soc Neurosci 5(4):401–416PubMedCrossRef Spengler S, von Cramon DY, Brass M (2010a) Resisting motor mimicry: control of imitation involves processes central to social cognition in patients with frontal and temporo-parietal lesions. Soc Neurosci 5(4):401–416PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Spengler S, Bird G, Brass M (2010b) Hyper-imitation of actions is related to reduced understanding of others’ minds in autism spectrum conditions. Biol Psychiatry 68(12):1148–1155 Spengler S, Bird G, Brass M (2010b) Hyper-imitation of actions is related to reduced understanding of others’ minds in autism spectrum conditions. Biol Psychiatry 68(12):1148–1155
go back to reference Teufel C, Fletcher PC, Davis G (2010) Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):376–382PubMedCrossRef Teufel C, Fletcher PC, Davis G (2010) Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception. Trends Cogn Sci 14(8):376–382PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Steenaert B, van Knippenberg A (2003a) Mimicry for money: behavioral consequences of imitation. J Exp Soc Psych 39(4):393–398CrossRef van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Steenaert B, van Knippenberg A (2003a) Mimicry for money: behavioral consequences of imitation. J Exp Soc Psych 39(4):393–398CrossRef
go back to reference van Baaren RB, Maddux WW, Chartrand TL, de Bouter C, van Knippenberg A (2003b) It takes two to mimic: behavioral consequences of self-construals. J Pers Soc Psychol 84(5):1093–1102PubMedCrossRef van Baaren RB, Maddux WW, Chartrand TL, de Bouter C, van Knippenberg A (2003b) It takes two to mimic: behavioral consequences of self-construals. J Pers Soc Psychol 84(5):1093–1102PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Kawakami K, van Knippenberg A (2004) Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol Sci 15(1):71–74PubMedCrossRef van Baaren RB, Holland RW, Kawakami K, van Knippenberg A (2004) Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychol Sci 15(1):71–74PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, Dapretto M (2006) Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Soc Cog Affect Neurosci 1(2):107–121CrossRef Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, Dapretto M (2006) Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Soc Cog Affect Neurosci 1(2):107–121CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Social attitudes differentially modulate imitation in adolescents and adults
Authors
Jennifer Cook
Geoffrey Bird
Publication date
01-06-2011
Publisher
Springer-Verlag
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 3-4/2011
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2584-4

Other articles of this Issue 3-4/2011

Experimental Brain Research 3-4/2011 Go to the issue