Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 5/2019

01-05-2019 | Research Article

Pre-crastination and procrastination effects occur in a reach-to-grasp task

Authors: Jarrod Blinch, Callie R. DeWinne

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 5/2019

Login to get access

Abstract

How do we decide which object to pick up when faced with two alternatives? Imagine one object is near, but needs to be carried a long distance, and the other object is far, but needs to be carried a short distance. You might predict that participants would favour the far object that needs to be carried a short distance. In other words, they would procrastinate and delay picking up an object to minimise physical effort. In actuality, participants prefer to carry the near object a long distance, which is called pre-crastination. Pre-crastination may be preferred to procrastination because picking up the first object hastens completion of the first goal of the task and, subsequently, decreases cognitive load. The goal of the current study was to further investigate the mechanisms of the pre-crastination effect. This was done by converting the primarily walking task used in the first study on pre-crastination to a reach-to-grasp task. This change enabled the measurement of the duration of information processing (i.e., reaction time) when participants decided which object to move. Surprisingly, participants exhibited a range of behaviours: about 40% pre-crastinated, 40% procrastinated, and 20% neither pre-crastinated nor procrastinated. We suggest that scaling the task down from a walking task to a reach-to-grasp task altered the physical effort, cognitive load, and the interaction between these task demands. This enabled some participants to pre-crastinate and others to procrastinate. There was an intriguing relationship between the duration of information processing and the behaviour of participants: participants with the shortest reaction time had the strongest tendency to pre-crastinate, and participants with the longest reaction time had the strongest tendency to procrastinate. These findings fit with the automatic pre-crastination response hypothesis; that the “decision” to pre-crastinate is automatic. This automaticity caused the short durations of information processing for participants who pre-crastinated. Participants who procrastinated had to, first, inhibit the automatic response to pre-crastinate, which caused long durations of information processing.
Footnotes
1
The reaction time results were comparable when the absolute tangential velocity threshold was replaced with a relative tangential velocity threshold of 5% of the initial reach to the selected block.
 
Literature
go back to reference Brown SG, Roy EA, Rohr LE, Bryden PJ (2006) Using hand performance measures to predict handedness. Laterality 11(1):1–14CrossRefPubMed Brown SG, Roy EA, Rohr LE, Bryden PJ (2006) Using hand performance measures to predict handedness. Laterality 11(1):1–14CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Carey DP, Hargreaves EL, Goodale MA (1996) Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control. Exp Brain Res 112(3):496–504CrossRefPubMed Carey DP, Hargreaves EL, Goodale MA (1996) Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control. Exp Brain Res 112(3):496–504CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Craighero L, Fadiga L, Umiltà CA, Rizzolatti G (1996) Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. Neuroreport 8(1):347–349CrossRefPubMed Craighero L, Fadiga L, Umiltà CA, Rizzolatti G (1996) Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. Neuroreport 8(1):347–349CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Klapp ST (1995) Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: the role of practice. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(5):1015–1027CrossRef Klapp ST (1995) Motor response programming during simple and choice reaction time: the role of practice. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(5):1015–1027CrossRef
go back to reference Klapp ST (2003) Reaction time analysis of two types of motor preparation for speech articulation: action as a sequence of chunks. J Mot Behav 35(2):135–150CrossRefPubMed Klapp ST (2003) Reaction time analysis of two types of motor preparation for speech articulation: action as a sequence of chunks. J Mot Behav 35(2):135–150CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Langner R, Steinborn MB, Chatterjee A, Sturm W, Willmes K (2010) Mental fatigue and temporal preparation in simple reaction-time performance. Acta Psychol 133(1):64–72CrossRef Langner R, Steinborn MB, Chatterjee A, Sturm W, Willmes K (2010) Mental fatigue and temporal preparation in simple reaction-time performance. Acta Psychol 133(1):64–72CrossRef
go back to reference Matthews G, Campbell SE, Falconer S, Joyner LA, Huggins J, Gilliland K, Grier R, Warm JS (2002) Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion 2(4):315–340CrossRefPubMed Matthews G, Campbell SE, Falconer S, Joyner LA, Huggins J, Gilliland K, Grier R, Warm JS (2002) Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion 2(4):315–340CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mon-Williams M, Tresilian JR, Coppard VL, Carson RG (2001) The effect of obstacle position on reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res 137(3–4):497–501PubMed Mon-Williams M, Tresilian JR, Coppard VL, Carson RG (2001) The effect of obstacle position on reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res 137(3–4):497–501PubMed
go back to reference Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1):97–113CrossRefPubMed Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1):97–113CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Potts CA, Callahan-Flintoft C, Rosenbaum DA (2018) How do reaching and walking costs affect movement path selection? Exp Brain Res 236:2727–2737CrossRef Potts CA, Callahan-Flintoft C, Rosenbaum DA (2018) How do reaching and walking costs affect movement path selection? Exp Brain Res 236:2727–2737CrossRef
go back to reference Rosenbaum DA, Gong L, Potts CA (2014) Pre-crastination: hastening subgoal completion at the expense of extra physical effort. Psychol Sci 25(7):1487–1496CrossRefPubMed Rosenbaum DA, Gong L, Potts CA (2014) Pre-crastination: hastening subgoal completion at the expense of extra physical effort. Psychol Sci 25(7):1487–1496CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stone KD, Bryant DC, Gonzalez CLR (2013) Hand use for grasping in a bimanual task: evidence for different roles? Exp Brain Res 224(3):455–467CrossRefPubMed Stone KD, Bryant DC, Gonzalez CLR (2013) Hand use for grasping in a bimanual task: evidence for different roles? Exp Brain Res 224(3):455–467CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tresilian JR (1998) Attention in action or obstruction of movement? A kinematic analysis of avoidance behavior in prehension. Exp Brain Res 120(3):352–368CrossRefPubMed Tresilian JR (1998) Attention in action or obstruction of movement? A kinematic analysis of avoidance behavior in prehension. Exp Brain Res 120(3):352–368CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tucker M, Ellis R (1998) On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24(3):830CrossRefPubMed Tucker M, Ellis R (1998) On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24(3):830CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Voudouris D, Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2012) Do obstacles affect the selection of grasping points? Hum Mov Sci 31(5):1090–1102CrossRefPubMed Voudouris D, Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2012) Do obstacles affect the selection of grasping points? Hum Mov Sci 31(5):1090–1102CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zentall TR, Case JP, Andrews DM (2018) Procrastination in the pigeon: can conditioned reinforcement increase the likelihood of human procrastination? Psychon Bull Rev 25(5):1952–1957CrossRefPubMed Zentall TR, Case JP, Andrews DM (2018) Procrastination in the pigeon: can conditioned reinforcement increase the likelihood of human procrastination? Psychon Bull Rev 25(5):1952–1957CrossRefPubMed
Metadata
Title
Pre-crastination and procrastination effects occur in a reach-to-grasp task
Authors
Jarrod Blinch
Callie R. DeWinne
Publication date
01-05-2019
Publisher
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 5/2019
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-019-05493-3

Other articles of this Issue 5/2019

Experimental Brain Research 5/2019 Go to the issue