Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 4/2013

01-04-2013 | Research Article

Numbers reorient visuo-spatial attention during cancellation tasks

Authors: Samuel Di Luca, Mauro Pesenti, Giuseppe Vallar, Luisa Girelli

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 4/2013

Login to get access

Abstract

Numbers induce shifts of spatial attention on the left or the right sides of external space as a function of their magnitude. However, whether this number–space association is restricted to the linear horizontal extensions, or extends to the whole visual scene, is still an open question. This study investigates, by means of a cancellation paradigm, the influence of numerical magnitude during scanning tasks in which participants freely explore complex visual scenes unconstrained towards either the horizontal or the vertical unidimensional axes. Five cancellation tasks were adapted in which Arabic digits were used as targets or distracters, in structured (lines and columns) or unstructured visual displays, with a smaller (2 or 3 types of distracters) or larger (10 or more types of distracters) sets of stimuli. Results show that the participants’ hits distribution was a function of number magnitude: shifted on the left for small and on the right for large numbers. This effect was maximised when numerical cues were sparse, randomly arranged and, critically, irrelevant to the task. Overall, this study provides novel evidence from visuo-spatial exploratory cancellation tasks for an attentional shift induced by number magnitude.
Literature
go back to reference Axenfeld D (1894) Eine einfache Methode Hemianopsie zu constatiren. Neurologisches Centralblatt 13:437–438 Axenfeld D (1894) Eine einfache Methode Hemianopsie zu constatiren. Neurologisches Centralblatt 13:437–438
go back to reference Bowers D, Heilman KM (1980) Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18:491–498CrossRefPubMed Bowers D, Heilman KM (1980) Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18:491–498CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cappelletti M, Freeman ED, Cipolotti L (2007) The middle house or the middle floor: bisecting horizontal or vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia 45(13):2989–3000CrossRefPubMed Cappelletti M, Freeman ED, Cipolotti L (2007) The middle house or the middle floor: bisecting horizontal or vertical mental number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia 45(13):2989–3000CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cattaneo Z, Silvanto J, Pascual-Leone A, Battelli L (2009) The middle range of the number line orients attention to the left side of visual space. Cogn Neuropsychol 26(3):235–246CrossRefPubMed Cattaneo Z, Silvanto J, Pascual-Leone A, Battelli L (2009) The middle range of the number line orients attention to the left side of visual space. Cogn Neuropsychol 26(3):235–246CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Cattaneo Z, Fantino M, Tinti C, Silvanto J, Vecchi T (2010) Crossmodal interaction between the mental number line and peripersonal haptic space representation in sighted and blind individuals. Psychon Soc 72(4):885–890 Cattaneo Z, Fantino M, Tinti C, Silvanto J, Vecchi T (2010) Crossmodal interaction between the mental number line and peripersonal haptic space representation in sighted and blind individuals. Psychon Soc 72(4):885–890
go back to reference Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale
go back to reference Dawes S, Senior G (2001) Australian normative data and clinical utility of the Mesulam and Weintraub Cancellation Test. In: Poster presented at the 21st annual conference of the National Academy of Neuropsychology, San Francisco, California, USA Dawes S, Senior G (2001) Australian normative data and clinical utility of the Mesulam and Weintraub Cancellation Test. In: Poster presented at the 21st annual conference of the National Academy of Neuropsychology, San Francisco, California, USA
go back to reference de Hevia MD, Girelli L, Vallar G (2006) Numbers and space: a cognitive illusion? Exp Brain Res 168:254–264CrossRef de Hevia MD, Girelli L, Vallar G (2006) Numbers and space: a cognitive illusion? Exp Brain Res 168:254–264CrossRef
go back to reference Doricchi F, Guariglia P, Gasparini M, Tomaiuolo F (2005) Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nat Neurosci 8(12):1663–1665CrossRefPubMed Doricchi F, Guariglia P, Gasparini M, Tomaiuolo F (2005) Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nat Neurosci 8(12):1663–1665CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Fischer MH, Castel AD, Dodd MD, Pratt J (2003) Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 6(6):555–556CrossRefPubMed Fischer MH, Castel AD, Dodd MD, Pratt J (2003) Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 6(6):555–556CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Friesen CK, Kingstone A (1998) The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychon Bull Rev 5:490–495CrossRef Friesen CK, Kingstone A (1998) The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychon Bull Rev 5:490–495CrossRef
go back to reference Galfano G, Pavani F (2005) Long-lasting capture of tactile attention by body shadows. Exp Brain Res 166:518–527CrossRefPubMed Galfano G, Pavani F (2005) Long-lasting capture of tactile attention by body shadows. Exp Brain Res 166:518–527CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Galfano G, Rusconi E, Umiltà C (2006) Number magnitude orients attention, but not against one’s will. Psychon Bull Rev 13:869–874CrossRefPubMed Galfano G, Rusconi E, Umiltà C (2006) Number magnitude orients attention, but not against one’s will. Psychon Bull Rev 13:869–874CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gevers W, Lammertyn J, Notebaert W, Verguts T, Fias W (2006) Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychol 122:221–233CrossRef Gevers W, Lammertyn J, Notebaert W, Verguts T, Fias W (2006) Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychol 122:221–233CrossRef
go back to reference Halligan P, Wilson B, Cockburn JA (1990) A short screen test for visual neglect in stroke patients. Int Disabil Stud 12(3):95–99CrossRefPubMed Halligan P, Wilson B, Cockburn JA (1990) A short screen test for visual neglect in stroke patients. Int Disabil Stud 12(3):95–99CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hommel B, Pratt J, Colzato L, Godjin R (2001) Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychol Sci 123:360–365CrossRef Hommel B, Pratt J, Colzato L, Godjin R (2001) Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychol Sci 123:360–365CrossRef
go back to reference Huang HC, Wang TY (2008) Visualized representation of visual search patterns for a visuospatial attention test. Behav Res Methods 40(2):383–390CrossRefPubMed Huang HC, Wang TY (2008) Visualized representation of visual search patterns for a visuospatial attention test. Behav Res Methods 40(2):383–390CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ifrah G (1981) Histoire universelle des chiffres. Robert Laffont, Paris Ifrah G (1981) Histoire universelle des chiffres. Robert Laffont, Paris
go back to reference Ito Y, Hatta T (2004) Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence for the SNARC effect. Mem Cogn 32:662–673CrossRef Ito Y, Hatta T (2004) Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: evidence for the SNARC effect. Mem Cogn 32:662–673CrossRef
go back to reference Jewell G, McCourt ME (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38:93–110CrossRefPubMed Jewell G, McCourt ME (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38:93–110CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jonides J (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In: Long JB, Baddley AD (eds) Attention and performance IX. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 187–203 Jonides J (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In: Long JB, Baddley AD (eds) Attention and performance IX. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 187–203
go back to reference Keus IM, Jenks KM, Schwarz W (2005) Psychophysical evidence that the SNRAC effect has its functional locus in the response selection stage. Cogn Brain Res 24:48–56CrossRef Keus IM, Jenks KM, Schwarz W (2005) Psychophysical evidence that the SNRAC effect has its functional locus in the response selection stage. Cogn Brain Res 24:48–56CrossRef
go back to reference Maass A, Suitner C, Favaretto X, Cignacchi M (2009) Groups in space: stereotypes and the spatial agency bias. J Exp Soc Psychol 45:495–504CrossRef Maass A, Suitner C, Favaretto X, Cignacchi M (2009) Groups in space: stereotypes and the spatial agency bias. J Exp Soc Psychol 45:495–504CrossRef
go back to reference Mapelli D, Rusconi E, Umiltà C (2003) The SNARC effect: an instance of the Simon effect? Cognition 88:B1–B10CrossRefPubMed Mapelli D, Rusconi E, Umiltà C (2003) The SNARC effect: an instance of the Simon effect? Cognition 88:B1–B10CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mark VW, Monson N (1997) Two-dimensional cancellation neglect: a review and suggested method of analysis. Cortex 33:553–562CrossRefPubMed Mark VW, Monson N (1997) Two-dimensional cancellation neglect: a review and suggested method of analysis. Cortex 33:553–562CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Mesulam MM (1985) Attention, confusional states, and neglect. In: Mesulam MM (ed) Principles of behavioural neurology. Davis, Philadelphia, pp 125–168 Mesulam MM (1985) Attention, confusional states, and neglect. In: Mesulam MM (ed) Principles of behavioural neurology. Davis, Philadelphia, pp 125–168
go back to reference Nicholls MER, Loftus AM, Gevers W (2008) Look, no hands: a perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention. Psychon Bull Rev 15(2):413–418CrossRefPubMed Nicholls MER, Loftus AM, Gevers W (2008) Look, no hands: a perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention. Psychon Bull Rev 15(2):413–418CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Pia L, Folegatti A, Guagliardo M, Genero R, Gindri P (2009) Are drawing perseverations part of the neglect syndrome? Cortex 45(3):293–299CrossRefPubMed Pia L, Folegatti A, Guagliardo M, Genero R, Gindri P (2009) Are drawing perseverations part of the neglect syndrome? Cortex 45(3):293–299CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Restle F (1970) Speed of adding and comparing numbers. J Exp Psychol 83:274–278CrossRef Restle F (1970) Speed of adding and comparing numbers. J Exp Psychol 83:274–278CrossRef
go back to reference Ristic J, Wright A, Kingstone A (2006) The number line effect reflects top-down control. Psychon Bull Rev 13(5):862–868CrossRefPubMed Ristic J, Wright A, Kingstone A (2006) The number line effect reflects top-down control. Psychon Bull Rev 13(5):862–868CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ronchi R, Algeri L, Chiapella L, Spada MS, Vallar G (2012) Spatial neglect and perseveration in visuo-motor exploration. Neuropsychology 26:588–603CrossRefPubMed Ronchi R, Algeri L, Chiapella L, Spada MS, Vallar G (2012) Spatial neglect and perseveration in visuo-motor exploration. Neuropsychology 26:588–603CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rossetti Y, Jacquin-Courtois S, Rode G, Ota H, Michel C, Boisson D (2004) Does action make the link between number and space representation? Visuo-manual adaptation improves number bisection in unilateral neglect. Psychol Sci 15:426–430CrossRefPubMed Rossetti Y, Jacquin-Courtois S, Rode G, Ota H, Michel C, Boisson D (2004) Does action make the link between number and space representation? Visuo-manual adaptation improves number bisection in unilateral neglect. Psychol Sci 15:426–430CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Schneider W, Eschmann A, Zuccolotto A (2002) E-Prime reference guide. Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh Schneider W, Eschmann A, Zuccolotto A (2002) E-Prime reference guide. Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh
go back to reference Schwarz W, Keus IM (2004) Moving the eyes along the mental number line: comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Percept Psychophys 66:651–664CrossRefPubMed Schwarz W, Keus IM (2004) Moving the eyes along the mental number line: comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Percept Psychophys 66:651–664CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Seron X, Pesenti M, Noël MP, Deloche G, Cornet JA (1992) Images of numbers, or “when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue”. Cognition 44:159–196CrossRefPubMed Seron X, Pesenti M, Noël MP, Deloche G, Cornet JA (1992) Images of numbers, or “when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue”. Cognition 44:159–196CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stoianov E, Kramer P, Umiltà C, Zorzi M (2008) Visuo-spatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition 106:770–779CrossRefPubMed Stoianov E, Kramer P, Umiltà C, Zorzi M (2008) Visuo-spatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition 106:770–779CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference To MP, Regan BC, Wood D, Mollon JD (2011) Vision out of the corner of the eye. Vis Res 51(1):203–214CrossRefPubMed To MP, Regan BC, Wood D, Mollon JD (2011) Vision out of the corner of the eye. Vis Res 51(1):203–214CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vallar G, Bolognini N (2012) Unilateral spatial neglect. In: Nobre AC, Kastner S (eds) The Oxford handbook of attention. Oxford University Press, Oxford Vallar G, Bolognini N (2012) Unilateral spatial neglect. In: Nobre AC, Kastner S (eds) The Oxford handbook of attention. Oxford University Press, Oxford
go back to reference Weintraub S, Mesulam MM (1985) Mental state assessment of young and elderly adults in behavioural neurology. In: Mesulam MM (ed) Principles of behavioural neurology. Davis, Philadelphia, pp 71–123 Weintraub S, Mesulam MM (1985) Mental state assessment of young and elderly adults in behavioural neurology. In: Mesulam MM (ed) Principles of behavioural neurology. Davis, Philadelphia, pp 71–123
go back to reference Zebian S (2005) Linkages between number, concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the reverse of SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. J Cogn Cult 5:165–190CrossRef Zebian S (2005) Linkages between number, concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: the SNARC effect and the reverse of SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. J Cogn Cult 5:165–190CrossRef
go back to reference Zorzi M, Priftis K, Umiltà C (2002) Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature 417:138–139CrossRefPubMed Zorzi M, Priftis K, Umiltà C (2002) Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature 417:138–139CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Zubin J (1935) Note on a transformation function for proportions and percentages. J Appl Psychol 19(2):213–220CrossRef Zubin J (1935) Note on a transformation function for proportions and percentages. J Appl Psychol 19(2):213–220CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Numbers reorient visuo-spatial attention during cancellation tasks
Authors
Samuel Di Luca
Mauro Pesenti
Giuseppe Vallar
Luisa Girelli
Publication date
01-04-2013
Publisher
Springer-Verlag
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 4/2013
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3393-0

Other articles of this Issue 4/2013

Experimental Brain Research 4/2013 Go to the issue