Skip to main content
Top
Published in: International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 2/2018

01-04-2018 | Original Article

Modulation of Physiological Responses as Indices of Attentional Bias in Dysphoria

Authors: Carlos Gantiva, Andrés Araujo, Narayana Aragão, Nohelia Hewitt

Published in: International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction | Issue 2/2018

Login to get access

Abstract

The experimental results on attentional bias in depression have been contradictory. Research on physiological correlates that validate attentional bias have been scarce. The aim of the present study was to evaluate physiological responses associated with attentional bias in dysphoria. Heart rate and the galvanic skin response were recorded in 30 dysphoric individuals and 30 nondysphoric individuals while they performed an emotional Stroop task. Dysphoric individuals exhibited attentional bias and heart rate deceleration in response to unpleasant words. Nondysphoric individuals exhibited attentional bias toward pleasant words, accompanied by a higher skin conductance response. These results suggest that heart rate deceleration in response to unpleasant words correlated with attentional bias in dysphoric individuals.
Footnotes
1
Pleasant words: kiss, orgasm, love, caress. Neutral words: pencil, paper, board, cell. Unpleasant words: war, bomb, abuse, torture.
 
Literature
go back to reference Barsky, A. J., Goodson, J. D., Lane, R. S., & Cleary, P. D. (1988). The amplification of somatic symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50(5), 510–519.CrossRefPubMed Barsky, A. J., Goodson, J. D., Lane, R. S., & Cleary, P. D. (1988). The amplification of somatic symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50(5), 510–519.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2013). Is emotional Stroop interference linked to affective responses? Evidence from skin conductance and facial electromyography. Emotion, 13(1), 129–138. doi:10.1037/a0029520.CrossRefPubMed Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2013). Is emotional Stroop interference linked to affective responses? Evidence from skin conductance and facial electromyography. Emotion, 13(1), 129–138. doi:10.​1037/​a0029520.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
go back to reference Brunoni, A. R., Kemp, A. H., Dantas, E. M., Goulart, A. C., Nunes, M. A., Boggio, P. S., et al. (2013). Heart rate variability is a trait marker of major depressive disorder: evidence from the sertraline vs. electric current therapy to treat depression clinical study. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 1937–1949. doi:10.1017/S1461145713000497.CrossRefPubMed Brunoni, A. R., Kemp, A. H., Dantas, E. M., Goulart, A. C., Nunes, M. A., Boggio, P. S., et al. (2013). Heart rate variability is a trait marker of major depressive disorder: evidence from the sertraline vs. electric current therapy to treat depression clinical study. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 1937–1949. doi:10.​1017/​S146114571300049​7.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Campbell, B. A., Wood, G., & McBride, T. (1997). Origins of orienting and defensive responses: An evolutionary perspective. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 41–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Campbell, B. A., Wood, G., & McBride, T. (1997). Origins of orienting and defensive responses: An evolutionary perspective. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 41–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..
go back to reference Critchley, H. D., Elliott, R., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(8), 3033–3040.CrossRefPubMed Critchley, H. D., Elliott, R., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(8), 3033–3040.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 1–11.CrossRef Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 1–11.CrossRef
go back to reference Fridrici, C., Driessen, M., Wingenfeld, K., Kremer, G., Kissler, J., & Beblo, T. (2014). Investigating biases of attention and memory for alcohol-related and negative words in alcohol-dependents with and without major depression after day-clinic treatment. Psychiatry Research, 218(3), 311–318. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.041.CrossRefPubMed Fridrici, C., Driessen, M., Wingenfeld, K., Kremer, G., Kissler, J., & Beblo, T. (2014). Investigating biases of attention and memory for alcohol-related and negative words in alcohol-dependents with and without major depression after day-clinic treatment. Psychiatry Research, 218(3), 311–318. doi:10.​1016/​j.​psychres.​2014.​03.​041.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gantiva, C., & Camacho, K. (2016). Características de la respuesta emocional generada por las palabras: Un estudio experimental desde la emoción y la motivación. Psychologia: Avances de la Disciplina, 10(2), 55–62. Gantiva, C., & Camacho, K. (2016). Características de la respuesta emocional generada por las palabras: Un estudio experimental desde la emoción y la motivación. Psychologia: Avances de la Disciplina, 10(2), 55–62.
go back to reference Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. C., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding the use of the Beck depression inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.CrossRef Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. C., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding the use of the Beck depression inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.CrossRef
go back to reference Klorman, R., Weissbert, R. P., & Wiessenfeld, A. R. (1977). Individual differences in fear and autonomic reactions to affective stimulation. Psychophysiology, 14, 45–51.CrossRefPubMed Klorman, R., Weissbert, R. P., & Wiessenfeld, A. R. (1977). Individual differences in fear and autonomic reactions to affective stimulation. Psychophysiology, 14, 45–51.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.446.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455. doi:10.​1037/​1528-3542.​5.​4.​446.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 219–225. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.004.CrossRefPubMed Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 219–225. doi:10.​1016/​j.​brat.​2009.​11.​004.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Maltzman, I., & Boyd, G. (1984). Stimulus significance and bilateral SCRs to potentially phobic pictures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 41–46.CrossRefPubMed Maltzman, I., & Boyd, G. (1984). Stimulus significance and bilateral SCRs to potentially phobic pictures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 41–46.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (Affective Norms for English Words). Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 600–605.CrossRefPubMed Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (Affective Norms for English Words). Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 600–605.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Richard-Devantoy, S., Ding, Y., Turecki, G., & Jollant, F. (2016). Attentional bias toward suicide-relevant information in suicide attempters: a cross-sectional study and a meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 196, 101–108. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.046.CrossRefPubMed Richard-Devantoy, S., Ding, Y., Turecki, G., & Jollant, F. (2016). Attentional bias toward suicide-relevant information in suicide attempters: a cross-sectional study and a meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 196, 101–108. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jad.​2016.​02.​046.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sanz, J., Navarro, M. E., & Vázquez, C. (2003). Adaptación española del Inventario para la Depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): Propiedades psicométricas en estudiantes universitarios. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 29(124), 239–288. Sanz, J., Navarro, M. E., & Vázquez, C. (2003). Adaptación española del Inventario para la Depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): Propiedades psicométricas en estudiantes universitarios. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 29(124), 239–288.
go back to reference Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Fisher, J. E., Silton, R. L., Stewart, J. L., Crocker, L. D., et al. (2014). Electrophysiological evidence of the time course of attentional bias in non-patients reporting symptoms of depression with and without co-occurring anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 301. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00301.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sass, S. M., Heller, W., Fisher, J. E., Silton, R. L., Stewart, J. L., Crocker, L. D., et al. (2014). Electrophysiological evidence of the time course of attentional bias in non-patients reporting symptoms of depression with and without co-occurring anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 301. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2014.​00301.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Macmillan. Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Macmillan.
go back to reference Storrie, M. C., Doerr, H. O., & Johnson, M. H. (1981). Skin conductance characteristics of depressed subjects before and after therapeutic intervention. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169(3), 176–179.CrossRefPubMed Storrie, M. C., Doerr, H. O., & Johnson, M. H. (1981). Skin conductance characteristics of depressed subjects before and after therapeutic intervention. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169(3), 176–179.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.CrossRef Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.CrossRef
go back to reference Toone, B. K., Cooke, E., & Lader, M. H. (1981). Electrodermal activity in the affective disorders and schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 11(3), 497–508.CrossRefPubMed Toone, B. K., Cooke, E., & Lader, M. H. (1981). Electrodermal activity in the affective disorders and schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 11(3), 497–508.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ward, N. G., Doerr, H. O., & Storrie, M. C. (1983). Skin conductance: a potentially sensitive test for depression. Psychiatry Research, 10(4), 295–302.CrossRefPubMed Ward, N. G., Doerr, H. O., & Storrie, M. C. (1983). Skin conductance: a potentially sensitive test for depression. Psychiatry Research, 10(4), 295–302.CrossRefPubMed
Metadata
Title
Modulation of Physiological Responses as Indices of Attentional Bias in Dysphoria
Authors
Carlos Gantiva
Andrés Araujo
Narayana Aragão
Nohelia Hewitt
Publication date
01-04-2018
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction / Issue 2/2018
Print ISSN: 1557-1874
Electronic ISSN: 1557-1882
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9774-7

Other articles of this Issue 2/2018

International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 2/2018 Go to the issue