Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Neuropsychology Review 2/2014

01-06-2014 | Review

What Neuropsychology Tells us About Human Tool Use? The Four Constraints Theory (4CT): Mechanics, Space, Time, and Effort

Author: François Osiurak

Published in: Neuropsychology Review | Issue 2/2014

Login to get access

Abstract

Our understanding of human tool use comes mainly from neuropsychology, particularly from patients with apraxia or action disorganization syndrome. However, there is no integrative, theoretical framework explaining what these neuropsychological syndromes tell us about the cognitive/neural bases of human tool use. The goal of the present article is to fill this gap, by providing a theoretical framework for the study of human tool use: The Four Constraints Theory (4CT). This theory rests on two basic assumptions. First, everyday tool use activities can be formalized as multiple problem situations consisted of four distinct constraints (mechanics, space, time, and effort). Second, each of these constraints can be solved by the means of a specific process (technical reasoning, semantic reasoning, working memory, and simulation-based decision-making, respectively). Besides presenting neuropsychological evidence for 4CT, this article shall address epistemological, theoretical and methodological issues I will attempt to resolve. This article will discuss how 4CT diverges from current cognitive models about several widespread hypotheses (e.g., notion of routine, direct and automatic activation of tool knowledge, simulation-based tool knowledge).
Footnotes
1
Some studies have assessed the apraxic patients’ ability to use several tools and objects together such as preparing instant coffee (multiple object task; e.g., De Renzi and Lucchelli 1988). These tasks are now considered as assessing the action disorganization syndrome because of the presence of several steps to achieve the goal (see below).
 
2
The notion of “space constraint” stresses the problem that tools and objects are not always here now, that is, directly present to our senses (e.g., they are not visible because they are in another room). In this way, the emphasis is put on the problem of the absence. This differs dramatically from the perspective where the space is divided in function of the individual’s capacities (e.g., reaching space). In this way, it has been shown that tool use can extend our reaching space (e.g., Farnè and Làdavas 2000; Cardinalli et al. 2009; Iriki et al. 1996). Nevertheless, even if an object is out of reach, this object can still be present to the senses, if it is visible, for example.
 
3
Several studies have revealed serious limitations on the ability of nonhuman animals to solve tool-use situations that are relatively simple for humans (e.g., Povinelli 2000; Santos et al. 2006; Visalberghi and Limongelli 1994). In line with this, it has been suggested that tool use in animals is not based on relational representations of the underlying generative mechanisms involved, but rather on “concrete” representations learned by trial-and-error (e.g., Penn et al. 2008). The notion of “basic processes” refers here to this kind of learning.
 
4
Interestingly, nonhuman, animal users fail transfer tasks (Martin-Ordas et al. 2008; see Penn et al. 2008). These findings provide support for the idea that humans are able to reinterpret the physical world in terms of unobservable, hypothetical entities, such as causal forces (Penn et al. 2008; see below).
 
5
The simulation-based knowledge is confronted with some limitations that will be discussed later.
 
6
The term technique is used here as synonymous with “mechanical knowledge”.
 
7
Semantic memory can be defined as “that system which processes, stores and retrieves information about the meaning of words, concepts and facts” (Warrington 1975). In other words, it allows us to think about things that are not here now (Tulving 1985).
 
8
Of course, this is not to deny that it is different to know that knives can generally be found in kitchens or supermarkets and to remember that we possess the appropriate knife in our kitchen or the supermarket where we used to go. This issue is at the heart of important controversies concerning the distinction between semantic and episodic memory introduced by Tulving (1972). Unfortunately, this issue is beyond the scope of the present article, so I will only refer to semantic knowledge to describe the ability to think about things that are not here now. Of particular interest here is that semantic knowledge is fundamental to categorize the tools and objects in function of different activities, from very basic (body self-care, feeding, sleeping) to “non-basic” activities (relaxing, playing the guitar, working). And, it is this categorical organization, which allows us to know and even to remember how to get the appropriate tools and objects. In other words, semantic memory is viewed here as the means to organize the search in memory.
 
9
This conception of working memory is quite consistent with current models (e.g., Baddeley 2003).
 
10
Tools can also be used to enhance cognitive skills (Jonassen 1992; see also Vygotsky 1978). For instance, abacuses and pocket calculators improve calculation efficiency, and diaries and portable phones facilitate remembering the activities to perform. These tools might suggest that cognitive effort is also assessed when people are engaged in everyday activities. Unfortunately, this issue is beyond the scope of the present article.
 
11
The simulation-based decision-making process has to be viewed as a dynamic process in that the effort associated with each option can be re-evaluated all along the activity. For instance, a user can choose to reach a ball that rolled under a sofa with the arm and then decide to get an umbrella to reach the ball with it.
 
12
As stressed above, a potential mistake might be to believe that when patients show how to use a tool in a conventional way, they necessarily access information about the usual function.
 
13
Recent evidence demonstrates that the perception of the length of rods via dynamic touch can be recalibrated after a training session with auditive feedback (Wagman and Abney 2012). Even if these results were not obtained in a tool-use situation they indicate that the perception of object properties is not specific to a particular perceptual modality but constrained by the object’s mechanical properties.
 
14
These procedures have been reported to be impaired in brain-damaged patients, particularly those with damage to superior parietal lobes (Binkofski et al. 2001). The term of tactile agnosia/apraxia have been proposed, stressing the importance of both the sensory and motor components of the disorder. Note also that, besides these stereotypical exploratory procedures, people can perceive a certain number of properties of objects (e.g., mass, length), without the benefit of vision, simply by wielding the objects. This kind of touch is referred to as dynamic touch (Turvey 1996; Wagman and Abney 2012). Interestingly, it has been shown that people are able, via dynamic touch, to determine whether an object is more suited for a given tool-use action (e.g., hammering) than for another one (e.g., poking; Wagman and Carello 2001; see also Wagman and Carello 2003).
 
15
Proponents of a strong, embodied cognition approach assume that, as people perceive visual tool or objects, simulations of potential actions can become automatically active in preparation for action (e.g., Barsalou 1999; Pezzulo et al. 2011). As discussed above, this proposal is debatable.
 
16
All human societies develop technical equipment, which is modified and improved, a phenomenon called ratchet effect (Tomasello 1999; for further discussion on this aspect with regard to the principles of dialectic and stigmergy, see Osiurak et al. 2010).
 
Literature
go back to reference Agre, P. E. (1988). The dynamic structure of everyday life (Tech. Rep. No. 1085). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Artificial Intelligence Laboratory. Agre, P. E. (1988). The dynamic structure of everyday life (Tech. Rep. No. 1085). Massachusetts Institute of Technology: Artificial Intelligence Laboratory.
go back to reference Andersen, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 1225–1232. Andersen, S. J., Yamagishi, N., & Karavia, V. (2002). Attentional processes link perception and action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 269, 1225–1232.
go back to reference Anderson, J. R., Bothwell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.PubMed Anderson, J. R., Bothwell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111, 1036–1060.PubMed
go back to reference Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use. London: Talyor & Francis. Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use. London: Talyor & Francis.
go back to reference Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.PubMed Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.PubMed
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMed
go back to reference Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.PubMed Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.PubMed
go back to reference Bartolo, A., Daumüller, M., Della Sala, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2007). Relationship between object-related gestures and the fractionated object knowledge system. Behavioural Neurology, 18, 143–147.PubMed Bartolo, A., Daumüller, M., Della Sala, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2007). Relationship between object-related gestures and the fractionated object knowledge system. Behavioural Neurology, 18, 143–147.PubMed
go back to reference Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal tool use behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. New York: Garland STPM Press. Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal tool use behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals. New York: Garland STPM Press.
go back to reference Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappel, J., Guthrie, C., & Cutting, N. (2011). Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition, 119, 301–306.PubMed Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappel, J., Guthrie, C., & Cutting, N. (2011). Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition, 119, 301–306.PubMed
go back to reference Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon. Bernstein, N. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon.
go back to reference Besnard, J., Allain, P., Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2009). Contrôle exécutif et comportement d’utilisation d’objets. Vers une dissociation. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 1, 120–132. Besnard, J., Allain, P., Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2009). Contrôle exécutif et comportement d’utilisation d’objets. Vers une dissociation. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 1, 120–132.
go back to reference Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Osiurak, F., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., et al. (2010). Utilization Behaviour: Clinical and theoretical approaches. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 1–10. Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Osiurak, F., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., et al. (2010). Utilization Behaviour: Clinical and theoretical approaches. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 1–10.
go back to reference Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2011). A contribution to the study of environmental dependency phenomena: The social hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3278–3294. Besnard, J., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2011). A contribution to the study of environmental dependency phenomena: The social hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3278–3294.
go back to reference Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096.PubMed Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076–1096.PubMed
go back to reference Binkofski, F., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2013). Two action systems in the human brain. Brain and Language, 127, 222–229.PubMed Binkofski, F., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2013). Two action systems in the human brain. Brain and Language, 127, 222–229.PubMed
go back to reference Binkofski, F., Kunesch, E., Classen, J., Seitz, R. J., & Freund, H.-J. (2001). Tactile apraxia. Unimodal disorder of tactile object exploration associated with parietal lesions. Brain, 124, 132–144. Binkofski, F., Kunesch, E., Classen, J., Seitz, R. J., & Freund, H.-J. (2001). Tactile apraxia. Unimodal disorder of tactile object exploration associated with parietal lesions. Brain, 124, 132–144.
go back to reference Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 237–242.PubMed Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 237–242.PubMed
go back to reference Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1981). Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: A preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 585–593. Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1981). Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees: A preliminary report. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 585–593.
go back to reference Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984). Possible causes of sex difference in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415–440. Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984). Possible causes of sex difference in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415–440.
go back to reference Boesch-Achermann, H., & Boesch, C. (1993). Tool use in wild chimpanzees: New light from dark forests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 18–21. Boesch-Achermann, H., & Boesch, C. (1993). Tool use in wild chimpanzees: New light from dark forests. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 18–21.
go back to reference Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.PubMed Borghi, A. M., & Riggio, L. (2009). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Research, 1253, 117–128.PubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.PubMed Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111, 395–429.PubMed
go back to reference Botvinick, M. M., & Rosen, Z. (2009). Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making. Psychological Research, 73, 835–842.PubMedCentralPubMed Botvinick, M. M., & Rosen, Z. (2009). Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making. Psychological Research, 73, 835–842.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neurosciences, 2, 236–251. Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neurosciences, 2, 236–251.
go back to reference Brazzelli, M., Colombo, N., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired cognitive abilities after bilateral frontal damage. Cortex, 30, 27–51.PubMed Brazzelli, M., Colombo, N., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired cognitive abilities after bilateral frontal damage. Cortex, 30, 27–51.PubMed
go back to reference Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: On the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 341–358.PubMed Bub, D. N., & Masson, M. E. J. (2010). Grasping beer mugs: On the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 341–358.PubMed
go back to reference Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor Apraxia: A call to action. Neurocase, 7, 445–448.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J. (2001). Ideomotor Apraxia: A call to action. Neurocase, 7, 445–448.PubMed
go back to reference Buxbaum, L. J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 201–218.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 201–218.PubMed
go back to reference Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219–254. Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Carew, T. G. (1997). The role of memory in object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 219–254.
go back to reference Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Montgomery, M. W. (1998). Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 617–643.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., & Montgomery, M. W. (1998). Ideational apraxia and naturalistic action. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 617–643.PubMed
go back to reference Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 226–239.PubMed Buxbaum, L. J., Kyle, K. M., & Menon, R. (2005). On beyond mirror neurons: Internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 226–239.PubMed
go back to reference Cardinalli, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19, 478–479. Cardinalli, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A. C., & Farnè, A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19, 478–479.
go back to reference Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate species, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition, 5, 71–78.PubMed Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate species, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Animal Cognition, 5, 71–78.PubMed
go back to reference Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMed Cho, D. T., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). The object-based Simon effect: Grasping affordance or relative location of the graspable part? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 853–861.PubMed
go back to reference Clark, M. A., Merians, A. S., Kothari, A., Poizner, H., Macauley, B., Rothi, L. J. G., et al. (1994). Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. Brain, 117, 1093–1106.PubMed Clark, M. A., Merians, A. S., Kothari, A., Poizner, H., Macauley, B., Rothi, L. J. G., et al. (1994). Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia. Brain, 117, 1093–1106.PubMed
go back to reference Cooper, R. P. (2002). Order and disorder in everyday action: The roles of contention scheduling and supervisory attentional system. Neurocase, 8, 61–79.PubMed Cooper, R. P. (2002). Order and disorder in everyday action: The roles of contention scheduling and supervisory attentional system. Neurocase, 8, 61–79.PubMed
go back to reference Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.PubMed Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.PubMed
go back to reference Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2006). Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 887–916.PubMed Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2006). Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. Psychological Review, 113, 887–916.PubMed
go back to reference Cooper, R. P., Schwartz, M. F., Yule, P. G., & Shallice, T. (2005). The simulation of action disorganisation in complex activities of daily living. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 959–1004.PubMed Cooper, R. P., Schwartz, M. F., Yule, P. G., & Shallice, T. (2005). The simulation of action disorganisation in complex activities of daily living. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 959–1004.PubMed
go back to reference Cubelli, R., Marchetti, C., Boscolo, G., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Cognition in action: Testing a model of limb apraxia. Brain and Cognition, 44, 144–165.PubMed Cubelli, R., Marchetti, C., Boscolo, G., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Cognition in action: Testing a model of limb apraxia. Brain and Cognition, 44, 144–165.PubMed
go back to reference Daprati, E., & Sirigu, A. (2006). How we interact with objects: Learning from brain lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 265–270.PubMed Daprati, E., & Sirigu, A. (2006). How we interact with objects: Learning from brain lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 265–270.PubMed
go back to reference De Renzi, E. (1989). Apraxia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 245–263). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. De Renzi, E. (1989). Apraxia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 245–263). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
go back to reference De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1988). Ideational apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173–1185.PubMed De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1988). Ideational apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173–1185.PubMed
go back to reference De Renzi, E., Cavalleri, F., & Facchini, S. (1996). Imitation and utilisation behaviour. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 61, 396–400.PubMedCentralPubMed De Renzi, E., Cavalleri, F., & Facchini, S. (1996). Imitation and utilisation behaviour. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 61, 396–400.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMed Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34, 35–42.PubMed
go back to reference Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 271–290. Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 271–290.
go back to reference Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645–1649.PubMed Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645–1649.PubMed
go back to reference Forde, E. M. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). The role of semantic knowledge and working memory in everyday tasks. Brain and Cognition, 44, 214–252.PubMed Forde, E. M. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2000). The role of semantic knowledge and working memory in everyday tasks. Brain and Cognition, 44, 214–252.PubMed
go back to reference Frey, S. H. (2008). Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 1951–1957. Frey, S. H. (2008). Tool use, communicative gesture and cerebral asymmetries in the modern human brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 1951–1957.
go back to reference Garcea, F. E., & Mahon, B. Z. (2012). What is a tool concept? Dissociating manipulation knowledge from function knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1303–1313. Garcea, F. E., & Mahon, B. Z. (2012). What is a tool concept? Dissociating manipulation knowledge from function knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1303–1313.
go back to reference Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin Co. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin Co.
go back to reference Gibson, K. R. (1993). Generative interplay between technical capacities, social relations, imitation and cognition. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 251–269). New York: Cambridge University Press. Gibson, K. R. (1993). Generative interplay between technical capacities, social relations, imitation and cognition. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 251–269). New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Giovannetti, T., Britnell, P., Brennan, L., Siderowl, A., Grossman, M., Libon, D. J., et al. (2012). Everyday action impairment in Parkinson’s disease dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18, 787–798.PubMedCentralPubMed Giovannetti, T., Britnell, P., Brennan, L., Siderowl, A., Grossman, M., Libon, D. J., et al. (2012). Everyday action impairment in Parkinson’s disease dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18, 787–798.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin: The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.PubMed Goldenberg, G. (1995). Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin: The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 33, 63–72.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G. (1999). Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in left or right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 37, 559–566.PubMed Goldenberg, G. (1999). Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in left or right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 37, 559–566.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G. (2013a). Apraxia: The cognitive side of motor control. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Goldenberg, G. (2013a). Apraxia: The cognitive side of motor control. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Goldenberg, G. (2013b). Apraxia in left-handers. Brain, 136, 2592–2601.PubMed Goldenberg, G. (2013b). Apraxia in left-handers. Brain, 136, 2592–2601.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581–589.PubMed Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1998). Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 36, 581–589.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.PubMed Goldenberg, G., & Spatt, J. (2009). The neural basis of tool use. Brain, 132, 1645–1655.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G., Hartmann, K., & Schlott, I. (2003). Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: Apraxia or asymbolia? Neuropsychologia, 41, 1565–1573.PubMed Goldenberg, G., Hartmann, K., & Schlott, I. (2003). Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: Apraxia or asymbolia? Neuropsychologia, 41, 1565–1573.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G., Hentze, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia. Neurology, 63, 1863–1867.PubMed Goldenberg, G., Hentze, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia. Neurology, 63, 1863–1867.PubMed
go back to reference Goldenberg, G., Hartmann-Schmid, K., Sürer, F., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2007). The impact of dysexecutive syndrome on use of tools and technical devices. Cortex, 43, 424–435.PubMed Goldenberg, G., Hartmann-Schmid, K., Sürer, F., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2007). The impact of dysexecutive syndrome on use of tools and technical devices. Cortex, 43, 424–435.PubMed
go back to reference Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task. Human Nature, 16, 211–232. Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task. Human Nature, 16, 211–232.
go back to reference Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed Greenwald, A. G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.PubMed
go back to reference Gruber, T., Clay, Z., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: Evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Animal Behaviour, 80, 1023–1033. Gruber, T., Clay, Z., & Zuberbühler, K. (2010). A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: Evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage. Animal Behaviour, 80, 1023–1033.
go back to reference Hartmann, K., Goldenberg, G., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: The neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia, 43, 625–627.PubMed Hartmann, K., Goldenberg, G., Daumüller, M., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: The neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia, 43, 625–627.PubMed
go back to reference Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–346.PubMed Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J., & Valenstein, E. (1982). Two forms of ideomotor apraxia. Neurology, 32, 342–346.PubMed
go back to reference Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J. G., Mack, L., Feinberg, T., & Watson, R. T. (1986). Apraxia after superior parietal lesions. Cortex, 32, 141–150. Heilman, K. M., Rothi, L. J. G., Mack, L., Feinberg, T., & Watson, R. T. (1986). Apraxia after superior parietal lesions. Cortex, 32, 141–150.
go back to reference Hermsdörfer, J., Hentze, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2006). Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1642–1652.PubMed Hermsdörfer, J., Hentze, S., & Goldenberg, G. (2006). Spatial and kinematic features of apraxic movement depend on the mode of execution. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1642–1652.PubMed
go back to reference Hodges, J. R., Spatt, J., & Patterson, K. (1999). “What” and “how”: Evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9444–9448. Hodges, J. R., Spatt, J., & Patterson, K. (1999). “What” and “how”: Evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9444–9448.
go back to reference Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 1913–1925.PubMed Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., & Spatt, J. (2000). The role of knowledge in object use: Evidence from semantic dementia. Brain, 123, 1913–1925.PubMed
go back to reference Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.PubMed
go back to reference Humphreys, G. W., & Forde, E. M. E. (1998). Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 771–811. Humphreys, G. W., & Forde, E. M. E. (1998). Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 771–811.
go back to reference Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.PubMed Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7, 2325–2330.PubMed
go back to reference Jacobs, D. H., Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J. G., Na, D. L., Gold, M., Foundas, A. L., et al. (1999). Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimer disease are dissociable. Neuropsychologia, 37, 875–880.PubMed Jacobs, D. H., Adair, J. C., Williamson, D. J. G., Na, D. L., Gold, M., Foundas, A. L., et al. (1999). Apraxia and motor-skill acquisition in Alzheimer disease are dissociable. Neuropsychologia, 37, 875–880.PubMed
go back to reference James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology. New York: Cosimo Classics (Original work published 1890) James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology. New York: Cosimo Classics (Original work published 1890)
go back to reference Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2012). On the persistence of tool- based compatibility effects. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 220, 16–22. Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2012). On the persistence of tool- based compatibility effects. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 220, 16–22.
go back to reference Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: More évidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex, 49, 2322–2333.PubMed Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: More évidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex, 49, 2322–2333.PubMed
go back to reference Jax, S. A., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2010). Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object. Cognition, 115, 350–355.PubMedCentralPubMed Jax, S. A., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2010). Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object. Cognition, 115, 350–355.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, 103–109. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, 103–109.
go back to reference Johnson, S. L. (1981). Effect of training device on retention and transfer of a procedural task. Human Factors, 23, 257–272. Johnson, S. L. (1981). Effect of training device on retention and transfer of a procedural task. Human Factors, 23, 257–272.
go back to reference Johnson-Frey, S. H. (2007). What puts the how in where? Tool use and the divided visual streams hypothesis. Cortex, 43, 368–375. Johnson-Frey, S. H. (2007). What puts the how in where? Tool use and the divided visual streams hypothesis. Cortex, 43, 368–375.
go back to reference Jonassen, D. H. (1992). What are cognitive tools? In P. A. M. Kommers, D. H. Jonassen, J. T. Mayes, & A. Ferreira (Eds.), Cognitive Tools for Learning (NATO ASI Series, pp. 1–6). Jonassen, D. H. (1992). What are cognitive tools? In P. A. M. Kommers, D. H. Jonassen, J. T. Mayes, & A. Ferreira (Eds.), Cognitive Tools for Learning (NATO ASI Series, pp. 1–6).
go back to reference Klatzky, R. L., & Lederman, S. J. (2002). Touch. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Experimental Psychology (pp. 147–176). New York: Wiley. Klatzky, R. L., & Lederman, S. J. (2002). Touch. In A. F. Healy & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Experimental Psychology (pp. 147–176). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682. Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682.
go back to reference Kunde, W., Müsseler, J., & Heuer, H. (2007). Spatial compatibility effects with tool use. Human Factors, 49, 661–670.PubMed Kunde, W., Müsseler, J., & Heuer, H. (2007). Spatial compatibility effects with tool use. Human Factors, 49, 661–670.PubMed
go back to reference Latash, M. L., Scholz, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2007). Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Motor Control, 11, 267–308. Latash, M. L., Scholz, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2007). Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Motor Control, 11, 267–308.
go back to reference Lauro-Grotto, R., Piccini, C., & Shallice, T. (1997). Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia. Cortex, 33, 593–622.PubMed Lauro-Grotto, R., Piccini, C., & Shallice, T. (1997). Modality-specific operations in semantic dementia. Cortex, 33, 593–622.PubMed
go back to reference Leakey, R. (1980). The making of Mankind. London: Book Club Associates. Leakey, R. (1980). The making of Mankind. London: Book Club Associates.
go back to reference Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.PubMed Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 342–368.PubMed
go back to reference Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1971). L’Homme et la matière. Paris: Albin Michel. Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1971). L’Homme et la matière. Paris: Albin Michel.
go back to reference Lesourd, M., Le Gall, D., Baumard, J., Croisile, B., Jarry, C., & Osiurak, F. (2013). Apraxia and Alzheimer’s disease: Review and perspectives. Neuropsychology Review, 23, 234–256.PubMed Lesourd, M., Le Gall, D., Baumard, J., Croisile, B., Jarry, C., & Osiurak, F. (2013). Apraxia and Alzheimer’s disease: Review and perspectives. Neuropsychology Review, 23, 234–256.PubMed
go back to reference Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilisation behaviour and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.PubMed Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilisation behaviour and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.PubMed
go back to reference Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19, 326–334.PubMed Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19, 326–334.PubMed
go back to reference Limongelli, L., Boysen, S. T., & Visalberghi, E. (1995). Comprehension of cause-effect relations in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 18–26.PubMed Limongelli, L., Boysen, S. T., & Visalberghi, E. (1995). Comprehension of cause-effect relations in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 18–26.PubMed
go back to reference Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–683. Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–683.
go back to reference Macuga, K. L., Papailiou, A. P., & Frey, S. H. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: Relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 169–174.PubMedCentralPubMed Macuga, K. L., Papailiou, A. P., & Frey, S. H. (2012). Motor imagery of tool use: Relationship to actual use and adherence to Fitts’ law across tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 169–174.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology, 102, 59–70.PubMed Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology, 102, 59–70.PubMed
go back to reference Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Concepts and categories: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 27–51.PubMedCentralPubMed Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2009). Concepts and categories: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 27–51.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86.PubMed Maravita, A., & Iriki, A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 79–86.PubMed
go back to reference Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: Great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Animal Cognition, 11, 423–430.PubMedCentralPubMed Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: Great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Animal Cognition, 11, 423–430.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Massen, C. (2013). Cognitive representations of tool-use interactions. New Ideas in Psychology, 31, 239–246. Massen, C. (2013). Cognitive representations of tool-use interactions. New Ideas in Psychology, 31, 239–246.
go back to reference Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Programming tool-use actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 692–704.PubMed Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2007). Programming tool-use actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 692–704.PubMed
go back to reference Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2349–2358. Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2349–2358.
go back to reference McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: Implications for human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: Implications for human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2006). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Morady, K., & Humphreys, G. (2011). Multiple tasks demands in action disorganization syndrome. Neurocase, 17, 461–472.PubMed Morady, K., & Humphreys, G. (2011). Multiple tasks demands in action disorganization syndrome. Neurocase, 17, 461–472.PubMed
go back to reference Morgado, N., Gentaz, E., Guinet, E., Osiurak, F., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2013). Within reach but no so reachable. Obstacles matter in visual perception of reaching distances. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 462–467. Morgado, N., Gentaz, E., Guinet, E., Osiurak, F., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2013). Within reach but no so reachable. Obstacles matter in visual perception of reaching distances. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 462–467.
go back to reference Mounoud, P. (1996). A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms: The shift from partial to whole representation. In A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: the age of reason and responsibility (pp. 85–110). Chicago: Chicago University Press. Mounoud, P. (1996). A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms: The shift from partial to whole representation. In A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: the age of reason and responsibility (pp. 85–110). Chicago: Chicago University Press.
go back to reference Negri, G. A., Lunardelli, A., Reverberi, C., Gigli, G. L., & Rumiati, R. I. (2007a). Degraded semantic knowledge and accurate object use. Cortex, 43, 376–388.PubMed Negri, G. A., Lunardelli, A., Reverberi, C., Gigli, G. L., & Rumiati, R. I. (2007a). Degraded semantic knowledge and accurate object use. Cortex, 43, 376–388.PubMed
go back to reference Negri, G. A. L., Rumiati, R. I., Zadini, A., Ukmar, M., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2007b). What is the role of motor stimulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 795–816.PubMed Negri, G. A. L., Rumiati, R. I., Zadini, A., Ukmar, M., Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2007b). What is the role of motor stimulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 795–816.PubMed
go back to reference Nonaka, T. (2013). Motor variability but functional specificity: The case of a C4 tetraplegic mouth calligrapher. Ecological Psychology, 25, 131–154. Nonaka, T. (2013). Motor variability but functional specificity: The case of a C4 tetraplegic mouth calligrapher. Ecological Psychology, 25, 131–154.
go back to reference Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–14. Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–14.
go back to reference Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers. Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130, 66–73. Osiurak, F., & Badets, A. (2014). Pliers, not fingers. Tool-action effect in a motor intention paradigm. Cognition, 130, 66–73.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Richard, I., & Le Gall, D. (2008a). Object usage and object utilization. A single-case study. Neurocase, 14, 169–183. Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Richard, I., & Le Gall, D. (2008a). Object usage and object utilization. A single-case study. Neurocase, 14, 169–183.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2008b). Different constraints on grip selection in brain-damaged patients. Object use versus object transport. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2431–2434. Osiurak, F., Aubin, G., Allain, P., Jarry, C., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2008b). Different constraints on grip selection in brain-damaged patients. Object use versus object transport. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2431–2434.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2009). Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model. Cortex, 45, 769–783. Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Allain, P., Aubin, G., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Richard, I., et al. (2009). Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model. Cortex, 45, 769–783.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning. Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.PubMed Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2010). Grasping the affordances, understanding the reasoning. Toward a dialectical theory of human tool use. Psychological Review, 117, 517–540.PubMed
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2011). Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use. Neuropsychologia, 49, 299–312. Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., & Le Gall, D. (2011). Re-examining the gesture engram hypothesis. New perspectives on apraxia of tool use. Neuropsychologia, 49, 299–312.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Baltenneck, N., Boudin, B., & Le Gall, D. (2012a). Make a gesture and I will tell you what you are miming. Pantomime recognition in healthy subjects. Cortex, 48, 584–592. Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Baltenneck, N., Boudin, B., & Le Gall, D. (2012a). Make a gesture and I will tell you what you are miming. Pantomime recognition in healthy subjects. Cortex, 48, 584–592.
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2012b). Tool use and perceived distance. When unreachable becomes sponatenously reachable. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 331–339.PubMed Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2012b). Tool use and perceived distance. When unreachable becomes sponatenously reachable. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 331–339.PubMed
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., & Le Gall, D. (2013a). Mechanical problem-solving in left brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1964–1972.PubMed Osiurak, F., Jarry, C., Lesourd, M., Baumard, J., & Le Gall, D. (2013a). Mechanical problem-solving in left brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1964–1972.PubMed
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Roche, K., Ramone, J., & Chainay, H. (2013b). Handing a tool to someone can take more time than using it. Cognition, 128, 76–81.PubMed Osiurak, F., Roche, K., Ramone, J., & Chainay, H. (2013b). Handing a tool to someone can take more time than using it. Cognition, 128, 76–81.PubMed
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Wagner, C., Djerbi, S., & Navarro, J. (2013c). Why do we use automatic tools? The priority of control over effort. Experimental Psychology, 60, 453–468.PubMed Osiurak, F., Wagner, C., Djerbi, S., & Navarro, J. (2013c). Why do we use automatic tools? The priority of control over effort. Experimental Psychology, 60, 453–468.PubMed
go back to reference Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., Vallet, G., Drot, M., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2014). Getting a tool gives wings: Underestimation of effort for tool use. Psychological Research, 78, 1–9.PubMed Osiurak, F., Morgado, N., Vallet, G., Drot, M., & Palluel-Germain, R. (2014). Getting a tool gives wings: Underestimation of effort for tool use. Psychological Research, 78, 1–9.PubMed
go back to reference Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., et al. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annuals of Neurology, 34, 594–602. Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J.-S., et al. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annuals of Neurology, 34, 594–602.
go back to reference Penfield, W., & Evans, J. (1935). The frontal lobe in man: A clinical study of maximum removals. Brain, 58, 115–133. Penfield, W., & Evans, J. (1935). The frontal lobe in man: A clinical study of maximum removals. Brain, 58, 115–133.
go back to reference Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–130.PubMed Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–130.PubMed
go back to reference Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: A dialog on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 5.PubMedCentralPubMed Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2011). The mechanics of embodiment: A dialog on embodiment and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 5.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2013). Computational grounded cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 612.PubMedCentralPubMed Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. J. (2013). Computational grounded cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 612.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–558. Phillips, J. C., & Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Visual Cognition, 9, 540–558.
go back to reference Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes. New York: Oxford University Press. Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.
go back to reference Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1, 110–122. Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.
go back to reference Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in distance perception. Psychological Science, 14, 409–428. Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in distance perception. Psychological Science, 14, 409–428.
go back to reference Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness (pp. 67–89). London: London Academic Press. Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness (pp. 67–89). London: London Academic Press.
go back to reference Rieger, M. (2004). Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 555–565.PubMed Rieger, M. (2004). Automatic keypress activation in skilled typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 555–565.PubMed
go back to reference Rieger, M., & Massen, C. (2014). Tool characteristics in imagery of tool actions. Psychological Research, 78, 10–17.PubMed Rieger, M., & Massen, C. (2014). Tool characteristics in imagery of tool actions. Psychological Research, 78, 10–17.PubMed
go back to reference Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMed Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A. (2008). Reaching while walking: Reaching distance costs more than walking distance. Psychonomic Bulletin & review, 15, 1100–1104. Rosenbaum, D. A. (2008). Reaching while walking: Reaching distance costs more than walking distance. Psychonomic Bulletin & review, 15, 1100–1104.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J., & Jorgensen, M. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Rosenbaum, D. A., Marchak, F., Barnes, H. J., Vaughan, J., Slotta, J., & Jorgensen, M. (1990). Constraints for action selection: Overhand versus underhand grips. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 321–342). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Barnes, H. J., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1992). Time course of movement planning: Selection of handgrips for object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1058–1073.PubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Vaughan, J., Barnes, H. J., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1992). Time course of movement planning: Selection of handgrips for object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1058–1073.PubMed
go back to reference Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.PubMed Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. J., Vaughan, J., & Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applications to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734.PubMed
go back to reference Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458. Rothi, L. J. G., Ochipa, C., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–458.
go back to reference Roy, E. A., & Square, P. A. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (pp. 111–161). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Roy, E. A., & Square, P. A. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (pp. 111–161). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
go back to reference Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (pp. 7–57). Cambridge: MIT Press. Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1986). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (pp. 7–57). Cambridge: MIT Press.
go back to reference Santos, L. R., Pearson, H. M., Spaepen, G. M., Tsao, F., & Hauser, M. (2006). Probing the limits of tool competence: Experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus). Animal Cognition, 9, 94–109.PubMed Santos, L. R., Pearson, H. M., Spaepen, G. M., Tsao, F., & Hauser, M. (2006). Probing the limits of tool competence: Experiments with two non-tool-using species (Cercopithecus aethiops and Saguinus oedipus). Animal Cognition, 9, 94–109.PubMed
go back to reference Schwartz, M. F. (1995). Re-examining the role of executive functions in routine action production. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 769, 321–335. Schwartz, M. F. (1995). Re-examining the role of executive functions in routine action production. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 769, 321–335.
go back to reference Schwartz, M. F., Reed, E. S., Montgomery, M. W., Palmer, C., & Mayer, N. H. (1991). The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 381–414. Schwartz, M. F., Reed, E. S., Montgomery, M. W., Palmer, C., & Mayer, N. H. (1991). The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 381–414.
go back to reference Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M. W., Buxbaum, L. J., Lee, S. S., Carew, T. G., Coslett, H. B., et al. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13–28.PubMed Schwartz, M. F., Montgomery, M. W., Buxbaum, L. J., Lee, S. S., Carew, T. G., Coslett, H. B., et al. (1998). Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 12, 13–28.PubMed
go back to reference Schwartz, M. F., Segal, M., Veramonti, T., Ferraro, M., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2002). The naturalistic action test: A standardized assessment for everyday action impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 311–339. Schwartz, M. F., Segal, M., Veramonti, T., Ferraro, M., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2002). The naturalistic action test: A standardized assessment for everyday action impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 311–339.
go back to reference Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741.PubMed Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741.PubMed
go back to reference Shallice, T., Burgess, P. W., Schon, F., & Baxter, D. M. (1989). The origins of utilisation behaviour. Brain, 112, 1587–1598.PubMed Shallice, T., Burgess, P. W., Schon, F., & Baxter, D. M. (1989). The origins of utilisation behaviour. Brain, 112, 1587–1598.PubMed
go back to reference Shaw, R. (2003). The agent-environment interface: Simon’s indirect or Gibson’s direct coupling. Ecological Psychology, 15, 37–106. Shaw, R. (2003). The agent-environment interface: Simon’s indirect or Gibson’s direct coupling. Ecological Psychology, 15, 37–106.
go back to reference Shaw, R., Turvey, M. T., & Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 159–226). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Shaw, R., Turvey, M. T., & Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 159–226). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Silveri, M. C., & Ciccarelli, N. (2009). Semantic memory in object use. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2634–2641.PubMed Silveri, M. C., & Ciccarelli, N. (2009). Semantic memory in object use. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2634–2641.PubMed
go back to reference Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327. Singh, D. (1970). Preference for bar pressing to obtain reward over freeloading in rats and children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 320–327.
go back to reference Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.PubMed Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J.-R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.PubMed
go back to reference Solomon, R. L. (1948). The influence of work on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 45, 1–40.PubMed Solomon, R. L. (1948). The influence of work on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 45, 1–40.PubMed
go back to reference Solway, A., & Botvinick, M. M. (2012). Goal-directed decision making as probabilistic inference: A computational framework and potential neural correlates. Psychological Review, 119, 120–154.PubMedCentralPubMed Solway, A., & Botvinick, M. M. (2012). Goal-directed decision making as probabilistic inference: A computational framework and potential neural correlates. Psychological Review, 119, 120–154.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Spatt, J., Bak, T., Bozeat, S., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge: Contributions to object usage in corticobasal degeneration. Journal of Neurology, 249, 601–608.PubMed Spatt, J., Bak, T., Bozeat, S., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge: Contributions to object usage in corticobasal degeneration. Journal of Neurology, 249, 601–608.PubMed
go back to reference Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Visual Cognition, 12, 1337–1361. Symes, E., Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2005). Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances. Visual Cognition, 12, 1337–1361.
go back to reference Tarte, R. D. (1981). Contrafreeloading in humans. Psychological Reports, 49, 859–866. Tarte, R. D. (1981). Contrafreeloading in humans. Psychological Reports, 49, 859–866.
go back to reference Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror system: An integrative review. Neuroscience & BioBehavioral Reviews, 37, 491–521. Thill, S., Caligiore, D., Borghi, A. M., Ziemke, T., & Baldassarre, G. (2013). Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror system: An integrative review. Neuroscience & BioBehavioral Reviews, 37, 491–521.
go back to reference Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 493–498. Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., & Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 493–498.
go back to reference Tolman, E. C. (1949). The nature and functioning of wants. Psychological Review, 56, 357–369.PubMed Tolman, E. C. (1949). The nature and functioning of wants. Psychological Review, 56, 357–369.PubMed
go back to reference Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529. Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529.
go back to reference Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMed Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.PubMed
go back to reference Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: New York Academic Press. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: New York Academic Press.
go back to reference Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 25, 1–12. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 25, 1–12.
go back to reference Turvey, M. T. (1996). Dynamic Touch. American Psychologist, 51, 1134–1152.PubMed Turvey, M. T. (1996). Dynamic Touch. American Psychologist, 51, 1134–1152.PubMed
go back to reference Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.PubMed Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.PubMed
go back to reference Valyear, K. F., Chapman, C. S., Gallivan, J. P., Mark, R. S., & Culham, J. C. (2011). To use or to move: Goal-set modulates priming when grasping real tools. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 125–142.PubMed Valyear, K. F., Chapman, C. S., Gallivan, J. P., Mark, R. S., & Culham, J. C. (2011). To use or to move: Goal-set modulates priming when grasping real tools. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 125–142.PubMed
go back to reference van der Steen, M. C., & Bongers, R. M. (2011). Joint angle variability and co-variation in a reaching with a rod task. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 411–422.PubMed van der Steen, M. C., & Bongers, R. M. (2011). Joint angle variability and co-variation in a reaching with a rod task. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 411–422.PubMed
go back to reference van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 19–41. van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 19–41.
go back to reference Vingerhoets, G., Vandamme, K., & Vercammen, A. (2009). Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances. Brain and Cognition, 69, 481–489.PubMed Vingerhoets, G., Vandamme, K., & Vercammen, A. (2009). Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances. Brain and Cognition, 69, 481–489.PubMed
go back to reference Vingerhoets, G., Acke, F., Alderweireldt, A. S., Nys, J., Vandemaele, P., & Achten, E. (2012). Cerebral lateralization of praxis in right- and left-handedness: Same pattern, different strength. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 763–777.PubMed Vingerhoets, G., Acke, F., Alderweireldt, A. S., Nys, J., Vandemaele, P., & Achten, E. (2012). Cerebral lateralization of praxis in right- and left-handedness: Same pattern, different strength. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 763–777.PubMed
go back to reference Vingerhoets, G., Alderweireldt, A. S., Vandemaele, P., Cai, Q., van der Haegen, L., Brysbaert, M., et al. (2013). Praxis and language are linked: Evidence from co-lateralization in individuals with atypical language dominance. Cortex, 49, 172–183.PubMed Vingerhoets, G., Alderweireldt, A. S., Vandemaele, P., Cai, Q., van der Haegen, L., Brysbaert, M., et al. (2013). Praxis and language are linked: Evidence from co-lateralization in individuals with atypical language dominance. Cortex, 49, 172–183.PubMed
go back to reference Visalberghi, E., & Limongelli, L. (1994). Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 15–22.PubMed Visalberghi, E., & Limongelli, L. (1994). Lack of comprehension of cause-effect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 15–22.PubMed
go back to reference Visalberghi, E., & Trinca, L. (1989). Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding. Primates, 30, 511–521. Visalberghi, E., & Trinca, L. (1989). Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding. Primates, 30, 511–521.
go back to reference Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Boston: Harvard University Press. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Boston: Harvard University Press.
go back to reference Wagman, J. B., & Abney, D. H. (2012). Transfer of recalibration from audition to touch: Modality independence as a special case of anatomical independence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 589–602.PubMed Wagman, J. B., & Abney, D. H. (2012). Transfer of recalibration from audition to touch: Modality independence as a special case of anatomical independence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 589–602.PubMed
go back to reference Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195. Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–195.
go back to reference Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 175–186. Wagman, J. B., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: The user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 175–186.
go back to reference Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMed Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635–657.PubMed
go back to reference Waters, R. H. (1937). The principle of least effort in learning. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 3–20. Waters, R. H. (1937). The principle of least effort in learning. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 3–20.
go back to reference Witt, J. K. (2011a). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201–206. Witt, J. K. (2011a). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201–206.
go back to reference Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156.PubMed Witt, J. K. (2011b). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serves as indirect measures of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148–1156.PubMed
go back to reference Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.PubMedCentralPubMed Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479–1492.PubMedCentralPubMed
go back to reference Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888.PubMed Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2005). Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 880–888.PubMed
go back to reference Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMed Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–1882.PubMed
go back to reference Zadikoff, C., & Lang, A. E. (2005). Apraxia in movement disorders. Brain, 128, 1480–1497.PubMed Zadikoff, C., & Lang, A. E. (2005). Apraxia in movement disorders. Brain, 128, 1480–1497.PubMed
Metadata
Title
What Neuropsychology Tells us About Human Tool Use? The Four Constraints Theory (4CT): Mechanics, Space, Time, and Effort
Author
François Osiurak
Publication date
01-06-2014
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Neuropsychology Review / Issue 2/2014
Print ISSN: 1040-7308
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6660
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9260-y

Other articles of this Issue 2/2014

Neuropsychology Review 2/2014 Go to the issue