Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies 3/2019

01-09-2019 | Original Paper

The Epistemology of the Near Miss and Its Potential Contribution in the Prevention and Treatment of Problem-Gambling

Author: Cătălin Bărboianu

Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies | Issue 3/2019

Login to get access

Abstract

The near-miss has been considered an important factor of reinforcement in gambling behavior, and previous research has focused more on its industry-related causes and effects and less on the gaming phenomenon itself. The near-miss has usually been associated with the games of slots and scratch cards, due to the special characteristics of these games, which include the possibility of pre-manipulation of award symbols in order to increase the frequency of these “engineered” near-misses. In this paper, we argue that starting from a basic mathematical description of the classical (by pure chance) near-miss, generalizable to any game, and focusing equally on the epistemology of its constitutive concepts and their mathematical description, we can identify more precisely the fallacious elements of the near-miss cognitive effects and the inadequate perception and representation of the observational–intentional “I was that close.” This approach further suggests a strategy of using non-standard mathematical knowledge of an epistemological type in problem-gambling prevention and cognitive therapies.
Footnotes
1
See Griffiths (1991) for a well-organized brief of these early results.
 
2
According to frustration theory, failing to reach a goal (like winning) causes frustration, which fuels ongoing behavior toward that goal. According to cognitive-regret theory, the frustration caused by a near-miss induces a form of cognitive regret, which can be eliminated by playing again.
 
3
Skin conductance levels.
 
4
Sin conductance responses.
 
5
It is sometimes called “built-in near-miss”.
 
6
This distortion is different in nature from the classical gambling fallacies regarding probability, randomness, etc. It is actually a lack of information (of an ethical type) which prevents a distinction in nature for a gaming phenomenon.
 
7
For our purposes, the order of the elements or the direction in which they appear is not important.
 
8
The definition can be generalized further to have more than one partial outcome missed, or, better, to have a certain ratio between the number of missed items and the total size of W. In other words, we can gradualize the near-miss. For the purpose of simplicity and because the further arguments do not depend on such particularities of the near-miss, we shall retain the definition with one item missed.
 
9
Physical near-misses would also be specific to other wheel games or physical-skill games like those based on throwing objects at a target. However, the results of the current section do not apply to physical near-misses.
 
10
And unfolded down to show values and symbols.
 
11
That is, the probability of one does not or does depend on the probability of the other, respectively.
 
12
The sample space is the set of all possible outcomes as elementary events (combinations of the same size in our context). The probability field consists of a sample space, the set of all subsets of this sample space (called the field of events, which is a Boolean algebra) and a probability-function defined on the field of events. The information available when measuring a gaming event in probability is actually the sample space at that moment, consisting of the possible outcomes. These are determined by taking into account the items out of play (cards already dealt, symbols already having occurred, and so on).
 
13
The denotation of difference in the last factor is set-theoretic, that is, the elements that are in the first set and not in the second.
 
14
Note that time reference (occurrence, moment, after, right after, before) in this part of the section has a probabilistic nature and is thus conventional. It amounts to the available probability information and not to placement on a real timeline, although this may be the case in particular games and situations.
 
15
In a chronological sense.
 
16
This is also specific to the classical gambling fallacy of perceiving randomness and independence of the events.
 
Literature
go back to reference Abbott, M. W., & Volberg, R. A. (2000). Taking the pulse on gambling and problem gambling in New Zealand: A report on phase one of the 1999 national prevalence survey. Wellington: Department of Internal Affairs, Government of New Zealand. Abbott, M. W., & Volberg, R. A. (2000). Taking the pulse on gambling and problem gambling in New Zealand: A report on phase one of the 1999 national prevalence survey. Wellington: Department of Internal Affairs, Government of New Zealand.
go back to reference Amstel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative non reward in non continuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 102–119.CrossRef Amstel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative non reward in non continuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 102–119.CrossRef
go back to reference Blaszczynski, A., & Silove, D. (1995). Cognitive and behavioral therapies for pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11(2), 195–220.CrossRefPubMed Blaszczynski, A., & Silove, D. (1995). Cognitive and behavioral therapies for pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11(2), 195–220.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Costello, F., & Watts, P. (2017). Explaining high conjunction fallacy rates: The probability theory plus noise account. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2), 304–321.CrossRef Costello, F., & Watts, P. (2017). Explaining high conjunction fallacy rates: The probability theory plus noise account. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(2), 304–321.CrossRef
go back to reference Dixon, M. J., Harrigan, K. A., Jarick, M., MacLaren, V., Fugelsang, J. A., & Sheepy, E. (2011). Psychophysiological arousal signatures of near-misses in slot machine play. International Gambling Studies, 11(3), 393–407.CrossRef Dixon, M. J., Harrigan, K. A., Jarick, M., MacLaren, V., Fugelsang, J. A., & Sheepy, E. (2011). Psychophysiological arousal signatures of near-misses in slot machine play. International Gambling Studies, 11(3), 393–407.CrossRef
go back to reference Gerstein, D., Volberg, R.A., Murphy, S., Toce, M., et al. (1999). Gambling impact and behavior study. In: Report to the national gambling impact study commission. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center at the University of Chicago. Gerstein, D., Volberg, R.A., Murphy, S., Toce, M., et al. (1999). Gambling impact and behavior study. In: Report to the national gambling impact study commission. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center at the University of Chicago.
go back to reference Griffiths, M. (1991). Psychobiology of the near-miss in fruit machine gambling. The Journal of Psychology, 125(3), 347–357.CrossRefPubMed Griffiths, M. (1991). Psychobiology of the near-miss in fruit machine gambling. The Journal of Psychology, 125(3), 347–357.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Griffiths, M. (1994). The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. Brithish Journal of Psychology, 85, 351–369.CrossRef Griffiths, M. (1994). The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. Brithish Journal of Psychology, 85, 351–369.CrossRef
go back to reference Harrigan, K. A. (2008). Slot machine structural characteristics: Creating near misses using high award symbol ratios. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6, 353–368.CrossRef Harrigan, K. A. (2008). Slot machine structural characteristics: Creating near misses using high award symbol ratios. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6, 353–368.CrossRef
go back to reference Harrigan, K. A. (2009). Slot machines: Pursuing responsible gaming practices for virtual reels and near misses. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 7, 68–83.CrossRef Harrigan, K. A. (2009). Slot machines: Pursuing responsible gaming practices for virtual reels and near misses. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 7, 68–83.CrossRef
go back to reference Hertwig, R., Barron, G., Weber, E. U., & Erev, I. (2004). Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychological Science, 15(8), 534–539.CrossRefPubMed Hertwig, R., Barron, G., Weber, E. U., & Erev, I. (2004). Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychological Science, 15(8), 534–539.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The ‘conjunction fallacy’ revisited: How intelligent inferences look like reasoning errors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(4), 275.CrossRef Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The ‘conjunction fallacy’ revisited: How intelligent inferences look like reasoning errors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(4), 275.CrossRef
go back to reference Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences. Scientific American, 246, 160–173.CrossRef Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences. Scientific American, 246, 160–173.CrossRef
go back to reference Leonard, C. A., & Williams, R. J. (2016). The relationship between gambling fallacies and problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(6), 694.CrossRefPubMed Leonard, C. A., & Williams, R. J. (2016). The relationship between gambling fallacies and problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(6), 694.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., & Wiers, R. W. (2008). How to push someone’s buttons: A comparison of four anger-induction methods. Cognition and Emotion, 22(2), 353–373.CrossRef Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., & Wiers, R. W. (2008). How to push someone’s buttons: A comparison of four anger-induction methods. Cognition and Emotion, 22(2), 353–373.CrossRef
go back to reference Parke, J., & Griffiths, M. (2004). Gambling addiction and the evolution of the “near miss”. Addiction Research & Theory, 12(5), 407–411.CrossRef Parke, J., & Griffiths, M. (2004). Gambling addiction and the evolution of the “near miss”. Addiction Research & Theory, 12(5), 407–411.CrossRef
go back to reference Pelletier, M., & Ladouceur, R. (2007). The effect of knowledge of mathematics on gambling behaviours and erroneous perceptions. International Journal of Psychology, 42(2), 134–140.CrossRef Pelletier, M., & Ladouceur, R. (2007). The effect of knowledge of mathematics on gambling behaviours and erroneous perceptions. International Journal of Psychology, 42(2), 134–140.CrossRef
go back to reference Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.CrossRef Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.CrossRef
go back to reference Stange, M., Grau, M., Osazuwa, S., Graydon, C., & Dixon, M. J. (2017a). Reinforcing small wins and frustrating near-misses: Further investigation into scratch card gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(1), 47–63.CrossRefPubMed Stange, M., Grau, M., Osazuwa, S., Graydon, C., & Dixon, M. J. (2017a). Reinforcing small wins and frustrating near-misses: Further investigation into scratch card gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(1), 47–63.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stange, M., Graydon, C., & Dixon, M. J. (2016). “I was that close”: Investigating players’ reactions to losses, wins, and near-misses on scratch cards. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(1), 187–203.CrossRefPubMed Stange, M., Graydon, C., & Dixon, M. J. (2016). “I was that close”: Investigating players’ reactions to losses, wins, and near-misses on scratch cards. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(1), 187–203.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Stange, M., Graydon, C., & Dixon, M. J. (2017b). Increased urge to gamble following near-miss outcomes may drive purchasing behaviour in scratch card gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(3), 867–879.CrossRefPubMed Stange, M., Graydon, C., & Dixon, M. J. (2017b). Increased urge to gamble following near-miss outcomes may drive purchasing behaviour in scratch card gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(3), 867–879.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Steenbergh, T. A., Whelan, J. P., Meyers, A. W., May, R. K., & Floyd, K. (2004). Impact of warning and brief intervention messages on knowledge of gambling risk, irrational beliefs and behavior. International Gambling Studies, 4(1), 3–16.CrossRef Steenbergh, T. A., Whelan, J. P., Meyers, A. W., May, R. K., & Floyd, K. (2004). Impact of warning and brief intervention messages on knowledge of gambling risk, irrational beliefs and behavior. International Gambling Studies, 4(1), 3–16.CrossRef
go back to reference Turner, N. E., Macdonald, J., & Somerset, M. (2008). Life skills, mathematical reasoning and critical thinking: A curriculum for the prevention of problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(3), 367–380.CrossRefPubMed Turner, N. E., Macdonald, J., & Somerset, M. (2008). Life skills, mathematical reasoning and critical thinking: A curriculum for the prevention of problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(3), 367–380.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293.CrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, R. J., & Connolly, D. (2006). Does learning about the mathematics of gambling change gambling behavior? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(1), 62–68.CrossRefPubMed Williams, R. J., & Connolly, D. (2006). Does learning about the mathematics of gambling change gambling behavior? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(1), 62–68.CrossRefPubMed
Metadata
Title
The Epistemology of the Near Miss and Its Potential Contribution in the Prevention and Treatment of Problem-Gambling
Author
Cătălin Bărboianu
Publication date
01-09-2019
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Gambling Studies / Issue 3/2019
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3602
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-09820-1

Other articles of this Issue 3/2019

Journal of Gambling Studies 3/2019 Go to the issue