Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies 1/2018

01-03-2018 | Original Paper

Implicit Valuation of the Near-Miss is Dependent on Outcome Context

Authors: Parker J. Banks, Matthew S. Tata, Patrick J. Bennett, Allison B. Sekuler, Aaron J. Gruber

Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies | Issue 1/2018

Login to get access

Abstract

Gambling studies have described a “near-miss effect” wherein the experience of almost winning increases gambling persistence. The near-miss has been proposed to inflate the value of preceding actions through its perceptual similarity to wins. We demonstrate here, however, that it acts as a conditioned stimulus to positively or negatively influence valuation, dependent on reward expectation and cognitive engagement. When subjects are asked to choose between two simulated slot machines, near-misses increase valuation of machines with a low payout rate, whereas they decrease valuation of high payout machines. This contextual effect impairs decisions and persists regardless of manipulations to outcome feedback or financial incentive provided for good performance. It is consistent with proposals that near-misses cause frustration when wins are expected, and we propose that it increases choice stochasticity and overrides avoidance of low-valued options. Intriguingly, the near-miss effect disappears when subjects are required to explicitly value machines by placing bets, rather than choosing between them. We propose that this task increases cognitive engagement and recruits participation of brain regions involved in cognitive processing, causing inhibition of otherwise dominant systems of decision-making. Our results reveal that only implicit, rather than explicit strategies of decision-making are affected by near-misses, and that the brain can fluidly shift between these strategies according to task demands.
Literature
go back to reference AGCO (2014). Electronic Gaming Equiptment Minimum Technical Standards. AGCO (Alcohol and Gaming Commission of Ontario). AGCO (2014). Electronic Gaming Equiptment Minimum Technical Standards. AGCO (Alcohol and Gaming Commission of Ontario).
go back to reference Amsel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 55(2), 102.CrossRefPubMed Amsel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 55(2), 102.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Balleine, B. W., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 48–69.CrossRefPubMed Balleine, B. W., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 48–69.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 152.CrossRefPubMed Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 152.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Boyd, S., Chua, L. O., & Desoer, C. A. (1984). Analytical foundations of volterra series. IMA Journal of Mathematical Control and Information, 1(3), 243–282.CrossRef Boyd, S., Chua, L. O., & Desoer, C. A. (1984). Analytical foundations of volterra series. IMA Journal of Mathematical Control and Information, 1(3), 243–282.CrossRef
go back to reference Brevers, D., He, Q., Xue, G., & Bechara, A. (2017). Neural correlates of the impact of prior outcomes on subsequent monetary decision-making in frequent poker players. Biological Psychology, 124, 30–38.CrossRefPubMed Brevers, D., He, Q., Xue, G., & Bechara, A. (2017). Neural correlates of the impact of prior outcomes on subsequent monetary decision-making in frequent poker players. Biological Psychology, 124, 30–38.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Chase, H. W., & Clark, L. (2010). Gambling severity predicts midbrain response to near-miss outcomes. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(18), 6180–6187.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Chase, H. W., & Clark, L. (2010). Gambling severity predicts midbrain response to near-miss outcomes. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(18), 6180–6187.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Clark, L., Crooks, B., Clarke, R., Aitken, M. R., & Dunn, B. D. (2012). Physiological responses to near-miss outcomes and personal control during simulated gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(1), 123–137.CrossRefPubMed Clark, L., Crooks, B., Clarke, R., Aitken, M. R., & Dunn, B. D. (2012). Physiological responses to near-miss outcomes and personal control during simulated gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(1), 123–137.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Clark, L., Lawrence, A. J., Astley-Jones, F., & Gray, N. (2009). Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry. Neuron, 61(3), 481–490.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Clark, L., Lawrence, A. J., Astley-Jones, F., & Gray, N. (2009). Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry. Neuron, 61(3), 481–490.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Clark, L., Studer, B., Bruss, J., Tranel, D., & Bechara, A. (2014). Damage to insula abolishes cognitive distortions during simulated gambling. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(16), 6098–6103.CrossRef Clark, L., Studer, B., Bruss, J., Tranel, D., & Bechara, A. (2014). Damage to insula abolishes cognitive distortions during simulated gambling. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(16), 6098–6103.CrossRef
go back to reference Clotfelter, C. T., & Cook, P. J. (1993). Notes: The “gambler’s fallacy” in lottery play. Management Science, 39(12), 1521–1525.CrossRef Clotfelter, C. T., & Cook, P. J. (1993). Notes: The “gambler’s fallacy” in lottery play. Management Science, 39(12), 1521–1525.CrossRef
go back to reference Côté, D., Caron, A., Aubert, J., Desrochers, V., & Ladouceur, R. (2003). Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 433–438.CrossRefPubMed Côté, D., Caron, A., Aubert, J., Desrochers, V., & Ladouceur, R. (2003). Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 433–438.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Daunizeau, J., Adam, V., & Rigoux, L. (2014). VBA: A probabilistic treatment of nonlinear models for neurobiological and behavioural data. PLoS Computational Biology 10 (1):e1003441. Daunizeau, J., Adam, V., & Rigoux, L. (2014). VBA: A probabilistic treatment of nonlinear models for neurobiological and behavioural data. PLoS Computational Biology 10 (1):e1003441.
go back to reference Daunizeau, J., Friston, K. J., & Kiebel, S. J. (2009). Variational bayesian identification and prediction of stochastic nonlinear dynamic causal models. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 238(21), 2089–2118.CrossRef Daunizeau, J., Friston, K. J., & Kiebel, S. J. (2009). Variational bayesian identification and prediction of stochastic nonlinear dynamic causal models. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 238(21), 2089–2118.CrossRef
go back to reference Dixon, M. J., Harrigan, K. A., Jarick, M., MacLaren, V., Fugelsang, J. A., & Sheepy, E. (2011). Psychophysiological arousal signatures of near-misses in slot machine play. International Gambling Studies, 11(3), 393–407.CrossRef Dixon, M. J., Harrigan, K. A., Jarick, M., MacLaren, V., Fugelsang, J. A., & Sheepy, E. (2011). Psychophysiological arousal signatures of near-misses in slot machine play. International Gambling Studies, 11(3), 393–407.CrossRef
go back to reference Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The canadian problem gambling index. Ottawa: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The canadian problem gambling index. Ottawa: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.
go back to reference Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Effects of loss aversion on post-decision wagering: Implications for measures of awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 352–363.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fleming, S. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Effects of loss aversion on post-decision wagering: Implications for measures of awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 352–363.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Graybiel, A. M. (2008). Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 359–387.CrossRefPubMed Graybiel, A. M. (2008). Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 359–387.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Guillaume, S., Jollant, F., Jaussent, I., Lawrence, N., Malafosse, A., & Courtet, P. (2009). Somatic markers and explicit knowledge are both involved in decision-making. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2120–2124.CrossRefPubMed Guillaume, S., Jollant, F., Jaussent, I., Lawrence, N., Malafosse, A., & Courtet, P. (2009). Somatic markers and explicit knowledge are both involved in decision-making. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2120–2124.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Gupta, S. (1990). Impulsivity/sociability and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning: A replication. Personality and Individual Differences, 11(6), 585–589.CrossRef Gupta, S. (1990). Impulsivity/sociability and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning: A replication. Personality and Individual Differences, 11(6), 585–589.CrossRef
go back to reference Habib, R., & Dixon, M. R. (2010). Neurobehavioural evidence for the “near-miss” effect in pathological gamblers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93(3), 313–328.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Habib, R., & Dixon, M. R. (2010). Neurobehavioural evidence for the “near-miss” effect in pathological gamblers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93(3), 313–328.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference Harrigan, K. A. (2008). Slot machine structural characteristics: Creating near misses using high award symbol ratios. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6(3), 353–368.CrossRef Harrigan, K. A. (2008). Slot machine structural characteristics: Creating near misses using high award symbol ratios. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6(3), 353–368.CrossRef
go back to reference Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679.CrossRefPubMed Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Ito, M., & Doya, K. (2009). Validation of decision-making models and analysis of decision variables in the rat basal ganglia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(31), 9861–9874.CrossRefPubMed Ito, M., & Doya, K. (2009). Validation of decision-making models and analysis of decision variables in the rat basal ganglia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(31), 9861–9874.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R., & Frith, C. (2000). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: A study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage, 12(6), 713–725.CrossRefPubMed Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R., & Frith, C. (2000). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: A study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage, 12(6), 713–725.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Jog, M. S., Kubota, Y., Connolly, C. I., Hillegaart, V., & Graybiel, A. M. (1999). Building neural representations of habits. Science, 286(5445), 1745–1749.CrossRefPubMed Jog, M. S., Kubota, Y., Connolly, C. I., Hillegaart, V., & Graybiel, A. M. (1999). Building neural representations of habits. Science, 286(5445), 1745–1749.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the“ near miss” and the“ big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 155.CrossRefPubMed Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the“ near miss” and the“ big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 155.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Knoch, D., Brugger, P., & Regard, M. (2005). Suppressing versus releasing a habit: Frequency-dependent effects of prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cerebral Cortex, 15(7), 885–887.CrossRefPubMed Knoch, D., Brugger, P., & Regard, M. (2005). Suppressing versus releasing a habit: Frequency-dependent effects of prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cerebral Cortex, 15(7), 885–887.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Kurucz, G., & Körmendi, A. (2012). Can we perceive near miss? An empirical study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(1), 105–111.CrossRefPubMed Kurucz, G., & Körmendi, A. (2012). Can we perceive near miss? An empirical study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(1), 105–111.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311.CrossRef Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311.CrossRef
go back to reference McNaughton, N., & Gray, J. A. (2000). Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61(3), 161–176.CrossRefPubMed McNaughton, N., & Gray, J. A. (2000). Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61(3), 161–176.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Noël, X., Bechara, A., Dan, B., Hanak, C., & Verbanck, P. (2007). Response inhibition deficit is involved in poor decision making under risk in nonamnesic individuals with alcoholism. Neuropsychology, 21(6), 778.CrossRefPubMed Noël, X., Bechara, A., Dan, B., Hanak, C., & Verbanck, P. (2007). Response inhibition deficit is involved in poor decision making under risk in nonamnesic individuals with alcoholism. Neuropsychology, 21(6), 778.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Odlaug, B., Chamberlain, S., Kim, S., Schreiber, L., & Grant, J. (2011). A neurocognitive comparison of cognitive flexibility and response inhibition in gamblers with varying degrees of clinical severity. Psychological Medicine, 41(10), 2111–2119.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Odlaug, B., Chamberlain, S., Kim, S., Schreiber, L., & Grant, J. (2011). A neurocognitive comparison of cognitive flexibility and response inhibition in gamblers with varying degrees of clinical severity. Psychological Medicine, 41(10), 2111–2119.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
go back to reference O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science, 304(5669), 452–454.CrossRefPubMed O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science, 304(5669), 452–454.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 95–102.CrossRefPubMed O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 95–102.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Osumi, T., & Ohira, H. (2009). Cardiac responses predict decisions: An investigation of the relation between orienting response and decisions in the ultimatum game. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74(1), 74–79.CrossRefPubMed Osumi, T., & Ohira, H. (2009). Cardiac responses predict decisions: An investigation of the relation between orienting response and decisions in the ultimatum game. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74(1), 74–79.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Otis, J., & Ley, R. (1993). The effects of frustration induced by discontinuation of reinforcement on force of response and magnitude of the skin conductance response. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(2), 97–100.CrossRef Otis, J., & Ley, R. (1993). The effects of frustration induced by discontinuation of reinforcement on force of response and magnitude of the skin conductance response. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(2), 97–100.CrossRef
go back to reference Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (2001). Dopamine, appetitive reinforcement, and the neuropsychology of human learning: An individual differences approach. In: Eliasz, A., Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Advances in Individual Differences Research (pp. 113–149). Lengerich, Germany: PABST Science Publisher. Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (2001). Dopamine, appetitive reinforcement, and the neuropsychology of human learning: An individual differences approach. In: Eliasz, A., Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Advances in Individual Differences Research (pp. 113–149). Lengerich, Germany: PABST Science Publisher.
go back to reference Reid, R. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2(1), 32–39.CrossRef Reid, R. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2(1), 32–39.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuster, R. H. (1969). A functional analysis of conditioned reinforcement. In D. Hendry (Ed.), Conditioned Reinforcement (pp. 192–234). Homewood: Dorsey Press. Schuster, R. H. (1969). A functional analysis of conditioned reinforcement. In D. Hendry (Ed.), Conditioned Reinforcement (pp. 192–234). Homewood: Dorsey Press.
go back to reference Skelin, I., Hakstol, R., VanOyen, J., Mudiayi, D., Molina, L. A., Holec, V., et al. (2014). Lesions of dorsal striatum eliminate lose-switch responding but not mixed-response strategies in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(10), 1655–1663.CrossRefPubMed Skelin, I., Hakstol, R., VanOyen, J., Mudiayi, D., Molina, L. A., Holec, V., et al. (2014). Lesions of dorsal striatum eliminate lose-switch responding but not mixed-response strategies in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(10), 1655–1663.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.
go back to reference Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May i have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–183.CrossRefPubMed Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May i have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–183.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Strickland, L. H., & Grote, F. W. (1967). Temporal presentation of winning symbols and slot-machine playing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 10.CrossRefPubMed Strickland, L. H., & Grote, F. W. (1967). Temporal presentation of winning symbols and slot-machine playing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 10.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge: MIT press. Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge: MIT press.
go back to reference Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253–266.CrossRefPubMed Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253–266.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Tricomi, E. M., Delgado, M. R., & Fiez, J. A. (2004). Modulation of caudate activity by action contingency. Neuron, 41(2), 281–292.CrossRefPubMed Tricomi, E. M., Delgado, M. R., & Fiez, J. A. (2004). Modulation of caudate activity by action contingency. Neuron, 41(2), 281–292.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference van Holst, R. J., Chase, H. W., & Clark, L. (2014). Striatal connectivity changes following gambling wins and near-misses: Associations with gambling severity. NeuroImage: Clinical, 5, 232–239.CrossRef van Holst, R. J., Chase, H. W., & Clark, L. (2014). Striatal connectivity changes following gambling wins and near-misses: Associations with gambling severity. NeuroImage: Clinical, 5, 232–239.CrossRef
go back to reference Walton, M. E., Devlin, J. T., & Rushworth, M. F. (2004). Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 1259–1265.CrossRefPubMed Walton, M. E., Devlin, J. T., & Rushworth, M. F. (2004). Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 1259–1265.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Wohl, M. J., & Enzle, M. E. (2003). The effects of near wins and near losses on self-perceived personal luck and subsequent gambling behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(2), 184–191.CrossRef Wohl, M. J., & Enzle, M. E. (2003). The effects of near wins and near losses on self-perceived personal luck and subsequent gambling behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(2), 184–191.CrossRef
go back to reference Wong, S. A., Thapa, R., Badenhorst, C. A., Briggs, A. R., Sawada, J. A., & Gruber, A. J. (2017). Opposing effects of acute and chronic d-amphetamine on decision-making in rats. Neuroscience, 345, 218–228.CrossRefPubMed Wong, S. A., Thapa, R., Badenhorst, C. A., Briggs, A. R., Sawada, J. A., & Gruber, A. J. (2017). Opposing effects of acute and chronic d-amphetamine on decision-making in rats. Neuroscience, 345, 218–228.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yechiam, E., & Hochman, G. (2013). Losses as modulators of attention: review and analysis of the unique effects of losses over gains. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 497.CrossRefPubMed Yechiam, E., & Hochman, G. (2013). Losses as modulators of attention: review and analysis of the unique effects of losses over gains. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 497.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(6), 464–476.CrossRefPubMed Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(6), 464–476.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Yu, R., Mobbs, D., Seymour, B., & Calder, A. J. (2010). Insula and striatum mediate the default bias. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(44), 14,702–14,707.CrossRef Yu, R., Mobbs, D., Seymour, B., & Calder, A. J. (2010). Insula and striatum mediate the default bias. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(44), 14,702–14,707.CrossRef
go back to reference Ziolkowski, S. (2016). The world count of gaming machines 2015. Sydney: Gaming Technologies Association. Ziolkowski, S. (2016). The world count of gaming machines 2015. Sydney: Gaming Technologies Association.
Metadata
Title
Implicit Valuation of the Near-Miss is Dependent on Outcome Context
Authors
Parker J. Banks
Matthew S. Tata
Patrick J. Bennett
Allison B. Sekuler
Aaron J. Gruber
Publication date
01-03-2018
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Gambling Studies / Issue 1/2018
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3602
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9705-3

Other articles of this Issue 1/2018

Journal of Gambling Studies 1/2018 Go to the issue