Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies 1/2015

01-03-2015 | Original Paper

Delusions of Expertise: The High Standard of Proof Needed to Demonstrate Skills at Horserace Handicapping

Authors: Matthew Browne, Matthew J. Rockloff, Alex Blaszcynski, Clive Allcock, Allen Windross

Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies | Issue 1/2015

Login to get access

Abstract

Gamblers who participate in skill-oriented games (such as poker and sports-betting) are motivated to win over the long-term, and some monitor their betting outcomes to evaluate their performance and proficiency. In this study of Australian off-track horserace betting, we investigated which levels of sustained returns would be required to establish evidence of skill/expertise. We modelled a random strategy to simulate ‘naïve’ play, in which equal bets were placed on randomly selected horses using a representative sample of 211 weekend races. Results from a Monte Carlo simulation yielded a distribution of return-on-investments for varying number of bets (N), showing surprising volatility, even after a large number of repeated bets. After adjusting for the house advantage, a gambler would have to place over 10,000 bets in individual races with net returns exceeding 9 % to be reasonably considered an expert punter (α = .05). Moreover, a record of fewer bets would require even greater returns for demonstrating expertise. As such, validated expertise is likely to be rare among race bettors. We argue that the counter-intuitively high threshold for demonstrating expertise by tracking historical performance is likely to exacerbate known cognitive biases in self-evaluation of expertise.
Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Footnotes
1
This bias against the player may take form of share-transaction fees, or the operator dividend incorporated into racing totalisator systems. By definition, at least 50 % (probably far more, since the distribution of expertise/knowledge tends to be positively skewed) of players participating in games with a skill component do not possess above-average knowledge or proficiency. Furthermore, above-average proficiency may not be sufficient to overcome the intrinsic bias against the player in the return schedule.
 
2
Note that, by this definition, expert strategies may still yield long-term negative aggregate returns, if the benefit conferred by the expert strategy is outweighed by the intrinsic weighting of the game against the player.
 
3
Equal prior probabilities assumes that non-expert players Other forms of random play may also be defined. Possibilities include using probabilities determined by starter number, starting odds, or by a particular player’s prior probabilities of with respect to starter or odds. These would generally result in a more sensitive benchmark for comparison (at the expense of increased complexity), so the impact of varying the random model might warrant further investigation.
 
4
Once again, more complex random models may be defined that place variable bet sizes, potentially matched according to the distribution of bet sizes for the particular play record under consideration. For clarity, we omit such elaborations in the present paper.
 
Literature
go back to reference Brownstein, A. L., Read, S. J., & Simon, D. (2004). Bias at the racetrack: Effects of individual expertise and task importance on predecision reevaluation of alternatives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(7), 891–904. doi:10.1177/0146167204264083.CrossRefPubMed Brownstein, A. L., Read, S. J., & Simon, D. (2004). Bias at the racetrack: Effects of individual expertise and task importance on predecision reevaluation of alternatives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(7), 891–904. doi:10.​1177/​0146167204264083​.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dixon, M. J., Fugelsang, J. A., MacLaren, V. V., & Harrigan, K. A. (2013). Gamblers can discriminate “tight” from “loose” electronic gambling machines. International Gambling Studies, 13(1), 98–111. doi:10.1080/14459795.2012.712151.CrossRef Dixon, M. J., Fugelsang, J. A., MacLaren, V. V., & Harrigan, K. A. (2013). Gamblers can discriminate “tight” from “loose” electronic gambling machines. International Gambling Studies, 13(1), 98–111. doi:10.​1080/​14459795.​2012.​712151.CrossRef
go back to reference Ehrlinger, J., & Dunning, D. (2003). How chronic self-views influence (and potentially mislead) estimates of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 5–17.CrossRefPubMed Ehrlinger, J., & Dunning, D. (2003). How chronic self-views influence (and potentially mislead) estimates of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 5–17.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Epstein, R. A. (2012). The theory of gambling and statistical logic. Waltham, MA: Academic Press. Epstein, R. A. (2012). The theory of gambling and statistical logic. Waltham, MA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Klenke, A. (2008). Probability theory: A comprehensive course. New York: Springer.CrossRef Klenke, A. (2008). Probability theory: A comprehensive course. New York: Springer.CrossRef
go back to reference Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121–1134.CrossRefPubMed Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121–1134.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference May, P. (2013). In search of the winning system. Compton: Racing Post & Raceform. May, P. (2013). In search of the winning system. Compton: Racing Post & Raceform.
go back to reference McMullan, J. L., & Kervin, M. (2012). Selling internet gambling: Advertising, new media and the content of poker promotion. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 10(5), 622–645. doi:10.1007/s11469-011-9336-3.CrossRef McMullan, J. L., & Kervin, M. (2012). Selling internet gambling: Advertising, new media and the content of poker promotion. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 10(5), 622–645. doi:10.​1007/​s11469-011-9336-3.CrossRef
go back to reference Meyer, G., Meduna, M. von, Brosowski, T., & Hayer, T. Is poker a game of skill or chance? A quasi-experimental study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 1–16. doi:10.1007/s10899-012-9327-8. Meyer, G., Meduna, M. von, Brosowski, T., & Hayer, T. Is poker a game of skill or chance? A quasi-experimental study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 1–16. doi:10.​1007/​s10899-012-9327-8.
go back to reference Mitrovic, D. V., & Brown, J. (2009). Poker mania and problem gambling: A study of distorted cognitions, motivation and alexithymia. Journal of gambling studies/co-sponsored by the National Council on Problem Gambling and Institute for the Study of Gambling and Commercial Gaming, 25(4), 489–502. doi:10.1007/s10899-009-9140-1. Mitrovic, D. V., & Brown, J. (2009). Poker mania and problem gambling: A study of distorted cognitions, motivation and alexithymia. Journal of gambling studies/co-sponsored by the National Council on Problem Gambling and Institute for the Study of Gambling and Commercial Gaming, 25(4), 489–502. doi:10.​1007/​s10899-009-9140-1.
go back to reference Mun, J. (2006). Modeling risk: Applying Monte Carlo simulation, real options analysis, forecasting, and optimization techniques. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Mun, J. (2006). Modeling risk: Applying Monte Carlo simulation, real options analysis, forecasting, and optimization techniques. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
go back to reference Richard, D. C. S., Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2013). The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of disordered gambling. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.CrossRef Richard, D. C. S., Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2013). The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of disordered gambling. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.CrossRef
go back to reference Waylen, A. E., Horswill, M. S., Alexander, J. L., & McKenna, F. P. (2004). Do expert drivers have a reduced illusion of superiority? Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7(4–5), 323–331. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2004.09.009.CrossRef Waylen, A. E., Horswill, M. S., Alexander, J. L., & McKenna, F. P. (2004). Do expert drivers have a reduced illusion of superiority? Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7(4–5), 323–331. doi:10.​1016/​j.​trf.​2004.​09.​009.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Delusions of Expertise: The High Standard of Proof Needed to Demonstrate Skills at Horserace Handicapping
Authors
Matthew Browne
Matthew J. Rockloff
Alex Blaszcynski
Clive Allcock
Allen Windross
Publication date
01-03-2015
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Gambling Studies / Issue 1/2015
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3602
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-013-9420-7

Other articles of this Issue 1/2015

Journal of Gambling Studies 1/2015 Go to the issue