Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Genetic Counseling 4/2007

01-08-2007 | Original Paper

Information Processing in the Context of Genetic Risk: Implications for Genetic-Risk Communication

Authors: Holly Etchegary, Colin Perrier

Published in: Journal of Genetic Counseling | Issue 4/2007

Login to get access

Abstract

Communicating genetic-risk information is fraught with difficulties, and there are no universally accepted guidelines for clinical practice. In this paper, we suggest that information-processing models may offer some guidance for the communication of genetic risk. The paper reviews selected literature from health and social psychology, including defensive reactions to threatening health information, the Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM) and Self Affirmation Theory. Ultimately, it presents the Heuristic-Systematic Model (HSM) of information processing as a useful perspective from which to view genetic-risk communication. Through our review of this literature, we identify some of the variables found to influence the systematic or heuristic processing of risk information and note their relevance to genetic counseling contexts. We suggest that systematic information processing is conducive to informed decision-making, as well as improved understanding of risk information. Clinical practice implications derived from our review of these literatures are noted.
Literature
go back to reference Abramsky, L., & Fletcher, O. (2002). Interpreting information: What is said, what is heard. A questionnaire study of health professionals and members of the public. Prenat Diagn, 22, 1188–1194.PubMedCrossRef Abramsky, L., & Fletcher, O. (2002). Interpreting information: What is said, what is heard. A questionnaire study of health professionals and members of the public. Prenat Diagn, 22, 1188–1194.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Armitage, C., & Conner, M. (2000). Social cognition models and health behaviour: A structured review. Psychol & Health, 15, 173–189.CrossRef Armitage, C., & Conner, M. (2000). Social cognition models and health behaviour: A structured review. Psychol & Health, 15, 173–189.CrossRef
go back to reference Bandura, A. (2004). Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ & Beh, 31(2), 143–164.CrossRef Bandura, A. (2004). Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ & Beh, 31(2), 143–164.CrossRef
go back to reference Berkowitz, L., & Cottingham, D. (1960). The interest value and relevance of fear arousing communication. J Abnormal Soc Psychol, 60, 30–40. Berkowitz, L., & Cottingham, D. (1960). The interest value and relevance of fear arousing communication. J Abnormal Soc Psychol, 60, 30–40.
go back to reference Berry, D., Michas, I., Gillie, T., & Forster, M. (1997). What do patients want to know about their medicines and what do doctors want to tell them? A comparative study. Psychol & Health, 12, 467–480.CrossRef Berry, D., Michas, I., Gillie, T., & Forster, M. (1997). What do patients want to know about their medicines and what do doctors want to tell them? A comparative study. Psychol & Health, 12, 467–480.CrossRef
go back to reference Berry, D., Michas, I., & Bersellini, E. (2002). Communicating information about medication side effects: Effects on satisfaction, perceived risk to health and intention to comply. Psychol & Health, 17, 247–267.CrossRef Berry, D., Michas, I., & Bersellini, E. (2002). Communicating information about medication side effects: Effects on satisfaction, perceived risk to health and intention to comply. Psychol & Health, 17, 247–267.CrossRef
go back to reference Berry, D., Michas, I., & Bersellini, E. (2003). Communicating information about medication: The benefits of making it personal. Psychol & Health, 18(1), 127–139.CrossRef Berry, D., Michas, I., & Bersellini, E. (2003). Communicating information about medication: The benefits of making it personal. Psychol & Health, 18(1), 127–139.CrossRef
go back to reference Bier, V. (2001). On the state of the art: Risk communication to the public. Rel Engin System Safety, 71, 139–150.CrossRef Bier, V. (2001). On the state of the art: Risk communication to the public. Rel Engin System Safety, 71, 139–150.CrossRef
go back to reference Bottorff, J., Ratner, P., Johnson, J., Lovato, C., & Joab, A. (1998). Communicating cancer risk information: The challenges of uncertainty. Pat Educ Counsel, 33(1), 67–81.CrossRef Bottorff, J., Ratner, P., Johnson, J., Lovato, C., & Joab, A. (1998). Communicating cancer risk information: The challenges of uncertainty. Pat Educ Counsel, 33(1), 67–81.CrossRef
go back to reference Braithwaite, D., Emery, H., Walter, F., Prevost, A., & Sutton, S. (2004). Psychological impact of genetic counseling for familial cancer: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Nat Can Inst, 96(2), 122–133.CrossRef Braithwaite, D., Emery, H., Walter, F., Prevost, A., & Sutton, S. (2004). Psychological impact of genetic counseling for familial cancer: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Nat Can Inst, 96(2), 122–133.CrossRef
go back to reference Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. Zanna, J. Olson, & C. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. Zanna, J. Olson, & C. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. Veleman & J. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford. Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. Veleman & J. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity and task importance on attitude judgment. J Per Soc Psychol, 66(3), 460–473.CrossRef Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity and task importance on attitude judgment. J Per Soc Psychol, 66(3), 460–473.CrossRef
go back to reference Chalmers, K., & Thomson, K. (1996). Coming to terms with the risk of breast cancer: Perceptions of women with primary relatives with breast cancer. Qual Health Res, 6(2), 256–282.CrossRef Chalmers, K., & Thomson, K. (1996). Coming to terms with the risk of breast cancer: Perceptions of women with primary relatives with breast cancer. Qual Health Res, 6(2), 256–282.CrossRef
go back to reference Chen, S., & Chaiken, S. (1999). The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 73–96). New York: Guilford. Chen, S., & Chaiken, S. (1999). The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 73–96). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1–73.CrossRef Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1–73.CrossRef
go back to reference Croyle, R., & Lerman, C. (1999). Risk communication in genetic testing for cancer susceptibility. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr, 25, 59–66.CrossRef Croyle, R., & Lerman, C. (1999). Risk communication in genetic testing for cancer susceptibility. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr, 25, 59–66.CrossRef
go back to reference d’Agincourt-Canning, L. (2005). The effect of experiential knowledge on construction of risk perception in hereditary breast/ovarian cancer. J Genet Couns, 14(1), 55–69.PubMedCrossRef d’Agincourt-Canning, L. (2005). The effect of experiential knowledge on construction of risk perception in hereditary breast/ovarian cancer. J Genet Couns, 14(1), 55–69.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ditto, P., & Croyle, R. (1995). Understanding the impact of risk factor test results: Insights from a basic research program. In R. Croyle (Ed.), Psychosocial effects of screening for disease prevention and detection (pp. 144–181). New York: Oxford University Press. Ditto, P., & Croyle, R. (1995). Understanding the impact of risk factor test results: Insights from a basic research program. In R. Croyle (Ed.), Psychosocial effects of screening for disease prevention and detection (pp. 144–181). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Dorval, M., Gauthier, G., Maunsell, E., Dugas, M., Rouleau, I., et al. (2005). No evidence of false reassurance among women with an inconclusive BRCA 1/2 genetic test result. Cancer, Epidemiol, Biomarkers & Prev, 14(12), 2862–2867.CrossRef Dorval, M., Gauthier, G., Maunsell, E., Dugas, M., Rouleau, I., et al. (2005). No evidence of false reassurance among women with an inconclusive BRCA 1/2 genetic test result. Cancer, Epidemiol, Biomarkers & Prev, 14(12), 2862–2867.CrossRef
go back to reference Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
go back to reference Etchegary, H., & Fowler, K. (2006). They had the right to know: Genetic risk and perceptions of responsibility. Submitted, Psychol & Health. Etchegary, H., & Fowler, K. (2006). They had the right to know: Genetic risk and perceptions of responsibility. Submitted, Psychol & Health.
go back to reference Evers-Kiebooms, G., & Decruyenaere, M. (1998). Predictive testing for Huntington's disease: A challenge for persons at risk and for professionals. Pat Educ Counsel, 35, 15–26.CrossRef Evers-Kiebooms, G., & Decruyenaere, M. (1998). Predictive testing for Huntington's disease: A challenge for persons at risk and for professionals. Pat Educ Counsel, 35, 15–26.CrossRef
go back to reference Foster, C., Watson, M., Moyhnihan, C., Ardern-Jones, A., & Eeles, R. (2002). Genetic testing for breast and ovarian cancer predisposition: Cancer burden and responsibility. J Health Psychol, 7(4), 469–484.PubMedCrossRef Foster, C., Watson, M., Moyhnihan, C., Ardern-Jones, A., & Eeles, R. (2002). Genetic testing for breast and ovarian cancer predisposition: Cancer burden and responsibility. J Health Psychol, 7(4), 469–484.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gibbons, F., & Gerrard, M. (1997). Health images and their effects on health behavior. In B. Buunk & F. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping and well being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 63–94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gibbons, F., & Gerrard, M. (1997). Health images and their effects on health behavior. In B. Buunk & F. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping and well being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 63–94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chaiken, S. (1997). Selective use of heuristic and systematic processing under defense motivation. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 23, 84–97.CrossRef Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chaiken, S. (1997). Selective use of heuristic and systematic processing under defense motivation. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 23, 84–97.CrossRef
go back to reference Gordon-Lubitz, R. (2003). Risk communication: Problems of presentation and understanding. JAMA, 289(1), 95.PubMedCrossRef Gordon-Lubitz, R. (2003). Risk communication: Problems of presentation and understanding. JAMA, 289(1), 95.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Green, J., Hewison, J., Bekker, H., Bryant, L., & Cuckle, H. (2004). Psychosocial aspects of Genetic screening of pregnant women and newborns: A systematic review. Health Technology Assessment, 8, 1–124. Green, J., Hewison, J., Bekker, H., Bryant, L., & Cuckle, H. (2004). Psychosocial aspects of Genetic screening of pregnant women and newborns: A systematic review. Health Technology Assessment, 8, 1–124.
go back to reference Griffin, R., Dunwoody, S., & Neuwirth, K. (1999). Proposed model of the relationship of risk-information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. Environ Res, 80, 230–245.CrossRef Griffin, R., Dunwoody, S., & Neuwirth, K. (1999). Proposed model of the relationship of risk-information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. Environ Res, 80, 230–245.CrossRef
go back to reference Griffin, R., Neuwirth, K., Giese, J., & Dunwoody, S. (2002). Linking the heuristic-systematic model and depth of processing. Comm Res, 29, 705–732.CrossRef Griffin, R., Neuwirth, K., Giese, J., & Dunwoody, S. (2002). Linking the heuristic-systematic model and depth of processing. Comm Res, 29, 705–732.CrossRef
go back to reference Hallowell, N., Stratham, H., & Murton, F. (1998). Women's understanding of their risk of developing breast/ovarian cancer before and after genetic counseling. J Genet Couns, 7, 345–364.PubMedCrossRef Hallowell, N., Stratham, H., & Murton, F. (1998). Women's understanding of their risk of developing breast/ovarian cancer before and after genetic counseling. J Genet Couns, 7, 345–364.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hallowell, N. (2006). Varieties of suffering: Living with the risk of ovarian cancer. Health, Risk & Society, 8(1), 9–26. Hallowell, N. (2006). Varieties of suffering: Living with the risk of ovarian cancer. Health, Risk & Society, 8(1), 9–26.
go back to reference Harris, P., & Napper, L. (2005). Self affirmation and the biased processing of threatening health-risk information. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 31(9), 1250–1263.CrossRef Harris, P., & Napper, L. (2005). Self affirmation and the biased processing of threatening health-risk information. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 31(9), 1250–1263.CrossRef
go back to reference Janis, I., & Terwilliger, R. (1962). An experimental study of psychological resistances to fear-arousing communications. J Abnormal Soc Psychol, 65, 403–410.CrossRef Janis, I., & Terwilliger, R. (1962). An experimental study of psychological resistances to fear-arousing communications. J Abnormal Soc Psychol, 65, 403–410.CrossRef
go back to reference Jemmott, J., Ditto, P., & Croyle, R. (1986) Judging health status: Effects of perceived prevalence and personal relevance. J Per Soc Psychol, 50, 899–905.CrossRef Jemmott, J., Ditto, P., & Croyle, R. (1986) Judging health status: Effects of perceived prevalence and personal relevance. J Per Soc Psychol, 50, 899–905.CrossRef
go back to reference Julian-Reynier, C., Welkenhuysen, M., Hagoel, L., Decruyenaere, M., Hopwood, P., & the CRISCOM Working Group. (2003). Risk communication strategies: State of the art and effectiveness in the context of cancer genetic services. Eur J Human Genet, 11, 725–736.CrossRef Julian-Reynier, C., Welkenhuysen, M., Hagoel, L., Decruyenaere, M., Hopwood, P., & the CRISCOM Working Group. (2003). Risk communication strategies: State of the art and effectiveness in the context of cancer genetic services. Eur J Human Genet, 11, 725–736.CrossRef
go back to reference Kahlor, L., Dunwoody, S., Griffin, R., Neuwirth, K., & Giese, J. (2003). Studying heuristic-systematic processing of risk communication. Risk Anal, 23(2), 355–368.PubMedCrossRef Kahlor, L., Dunwoody, S., Griffin, R., Neuwirth, K., & Giese, J. (2003). Studying heuristic-systematic processing of risk communication. Risk Anal, 23(2), 355–368.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kenen, R., Ardern-Jones, A., & Eeles, R. (2003). Family stories and the use of heuristics: Women from suspected hereditary breast and ovarian cancer families. Soc Health & Ill, 25(7), 838–865.CrossRef Kenen, R., Ardern-Jones, A., & Eeles, R. (2003). Family stories and the use of heuristics: Women from suspected hereditary breast and ovarian cancer families. Soc Health & Ill, 25(7), 838–865.CrossRef
go back to reference Klein, W., & Weinstein, N. (1997). Social comparison and unrealistic optimism about personal risk. In B. Buunk & F. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping and well being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 25–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Klein, W., & Weinstein, N. (1997). Social comparison and unrealistic optimism about personal risk. In B. Buunk & F. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping and well being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 25–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Kunda, Z (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. J Per Soc Psychol, 53, 636–647.CrossRef Kunda, Z (1987). Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. J Per Soc Psychol, 53, 636–647.CrossRef
go back to reference Lerman, C., Lustbader, E., Rimer, B., Daly, M., Miller, S., Sands, C. et al. (1995). Effects of individualized breast cancer risk counseling: A randomized trial. J Nat Cancer Inst, 87, 286–292.PubMedCrossRef Lerman, C., Lustbader, E., Rimer, B., Daly, M., Miller, S., Sands, C. et al. (1995). Effects of individualized breast cancer risk counseling: A randomized trial. J Nat Cancer Inst, 87, 286–292.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Liberman, A., & Chaiken, S. (1992). Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages. Per Soc Psychol Bul, 18(6), 669–679.CrossRef Liberman, A., & Chaiken, S. (1992). Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages. Per Soc Psychol Bul, 18(6), 669–679.CrossRef
go back to reference Lowenstein, G., Weber, E., Hsee, C., & Welch, E. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull, 127, 267–286.CrossRef Lowenstein, G., Weber, E., Hsee, C., & Welch, E. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull, 127, 267–286.CrossRef
go back to reference Maheswaran, D., & Chaiken, S. (1991). Promoting systematic processing in low-motivation settings: Effect of incongruent information on processing and judgment. J Per Soc Psychol, 61, 13–25.CrossRef Maheswaran, D., & Chaiken, S. (1991). Promoting systematic processing in low-motivation settings: Effect of incongruent information on processing and judgment. J Per Soc Psychol, 61, 13–25.CrossRef
go back to reference Marteau, T., & Dormandy, E. (2001). Facilitating informed choice in prenatal testing: How well are we doing? Am J Med Genet, 106, 185–190.PubMedCrossRef Marteau, T., & Dormandy, E. (2001). Facilitating informed choice in prenatal testing: How well are we doing? Am J Med Genet, 106, 185–190.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McAllister, M. (2002). Predictive genetic testing and beyond: A theory of engagement. J Health Psychol, 7(5), 491–508.PubMedCrossRef McAllister, M. (2002). Predictive genetic testing and beyond: A theory of engagement. J Health Psychol, 7(5), 491–508.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mottram, D., & Reed, C. (1997). Comparative evaluation of patient information leaflets by pharmacists, doctors and the general public. J Clin Phar Ther, 22, 127–134.CrossRef Mottram, D., & Reed, C. (1997). Comparative evaluation of patient information leaflets by pharmacists, doctors and the general public. J Clin Phar Ther, 22, 127–134.CrossRef
go back to reference Natter, H., & Berry, D. (2005). Effects of active information processing on the understanding of risk information. Appl Cognit Psychol, 19, 123–135.CrossRef Natter, H., & Berry, D. (2005). Effects of active information processing on the understanding of risk information. Appl Cognit Psychol, 19, 123–135.CrossRef
go back to reference Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
go back to reference Reed, M., & Aspinwall, L. (1998). Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Mot & Emot, 22(2), 99–132. Reed, M., & Aspinwall, L. (1998). Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Mot & Emot, 22(2), 99–132.
go back to reference Rebbeck, T., Lynch, H., Neuhausen, S., Narod, S., van't Veer, L. et al. (2002). Prophylactic oophorectomy in carriers of BRCA 1 or BRCA 2 mutations. N Engl J Med, 346, 1616–1622.PubMedCrossRef Rebbeck, T., Lynch, H., Neuhausen, S., Narod, S., van't Veer, L. et al. (2002). Prophylactic oophorectomy in carriers of BRCA 1 or BRCA 2 mutations. N Engl J Med, 346, 1616–1622.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rees, G., Fry, A., & Cull, A. (2001). A family history of breast cancer: Women's experiences from a theoretical perspective. Soc Sci Med, 52, 1433–1440.PubMedCrossRef Rees, G., Fry, A., & Cull, A. (2001). A family history of breast cancer: Women's experiences from a theoretical perspective. Soc Sci Med, 52, 1433–1440.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Renner, B. (2004). Biased reasoning: Adaptive responses to health risk feedback. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 30(3), 384–396.CrossRef Renner, B. (2004). Biased reasoning: Adaptive responses to health risk feedback. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 30(3), 384–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Richards, M. P. M. (1996). Families, kinship and genetics. In T. Marteau & M. P. M. Richards (Eds.), The troubled helix: Social and psychological implications of the new human genetics (pp. 247–273). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Richards, M. P. M. (1996). Families, kinship and genetics. In T. Marteau & M. P. M. Richards (Eds.), The troubled helix: Social and psychological implications of the new human genetics (pp. 247–273). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Rothman, A., & Kiviniemi, M. (1999). Treating people with information: An analysis and review of approaches to communicating health risk information. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr, 25, 44–51.CrossRef Rothman, A., & Kiviniemi, M. (1999). Treating people with information: An analysis and review of approaches to communicating health risk information. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr, 25, 44–51.CrossRef
go back to reference Sherman, D., Nelson, L., & Steele, C. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 26(9), 1046–1058.CrossRef Sherman, D., Nelson, L., & Steele, C. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Per Soc Psychol Bull, 26(9), 1046–1058.CrossRef
go back to reference Sherman, D., & Cohen, G. (2002). Accepting threatening information: Self-affirmation and the reduction of defensive biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(4), 119–123.CrossRef Sherman, D., & Cohen, G. (2002). Accepting threatening information: Self-affirmation and the reduction of defensive biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(4), 119–123.CrossRef
go back to reference Shiloh, S. (1994). Heuristics and biases in health decision-making: Their expression in genetic counseling. In L. Heath, R. S. Tindale, J. Edwards, E. J. Posavac, F. B. Bryant, E. Henderson-King, Y. Suarez-Balcazar, & J. Myers (Eds.), Applications of heuristics and biases to social issues. London: Plenum Press. Shiloh, S. (1994). Heuristics and biases in health decision-making: Their expression in genetic counseling. In L. Heath, R. S. Tindale, J. Edwards, E. J. Posavac, F. B. Bryant, E. Henderson-King, Y. Suarez-Balcazar, & J. Myers (Eds.), Applications of heuristics and biases to social issues. London: Plenum Press.
go back to reference Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk and rationality. Risk Anal, 24(2), 311–322.PubMedCrossRef Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk and rationality. Risk Anal, 24(2), 311–322.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Steele, C. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). New York: Academic Press. Steele, C. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). New York: Academic Press.
go back to reference Taylor, S. (2004). Predictive genetic test decisions for Huntington's disease: Context, appraisal and new moral imperatives. Soc Sci & Med, 58(1), 137–149.CrossRef Taylor, S. (2004). Predictive genetic test decisions for Huntington's disease: Context, appraisal and new moral imperatives. Soc Sci & Med, 58(1), 137–149.CrossRef
go back to reference Trepanier, A., Ahrens, M., McKinnon, W., Peters, J., Stopfer, J., et al. (2004). Genetic cancer risk assessment and counseling: Recommendations of the National Society of Genetic Counselors. J Genet Couns, 13(2), 83–114.PubMedCrossRef Trepanier, A., Ahrens, M., McKinnon, W., Peters, J., Stopfer, J., et al. (2004). Genetic cancer risk assessment and counseling: Recommendations of the National Society of Genetic Counselors. J Genet Couns, 13(2), 83–114.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Trumbo, C. (1999). Heuristic-systematic information processing and risk judgment. Risk Anal, 19(3), 391–400.PubMed Trumbo, C. (1999). Heuristic-systematic information processing and risk judgment. Risk Anal, 19(3), 391–400.PubMed
go back to reference Trumbo, C. (2002). Information processing and risk perception: An adaptation of the heuristic-systematic model. J Comm, 52(2), 367–382.CrossRef Trumbo, C. (2002). Information processing and risk perception: An adaptation of the heuristic-systematic model. J Comm, 52(2), 367–382.CrossRef
go back to reference Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.PubMedCrossRef Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference van den Berg, M., Timmermans, D., ten Kate, L., van Vugt, J., & van der Wal, G. (2006). Informed decision making in the context of prenatal screening. Pat Educ & Counsel, 63, 110–117.CrossRef van den Berg, M., Timmermans, D., ten Kate, L., van Vugt, J., & van der Wal, G. (2006). Informed decision making in the context of prenatal screening. Pat Educ & Counsel, 63, 110–117.CrossRef
go back to reference Wadey, V., & Frank, C. (1997). The effectiveness of patient verbalization on informed consent. Canadian J Surgery, 40, 124–128. Wadey, V., & Frank, C. (1997). The effectiveness of patient verbalization on informed consent. Canadian J Surgery, 40, 124–128.
go back to reference Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Comm Mono, 59, 329–349.CrossRef Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Comm Mono, 59, 329–349.CrossRef
go back to reference Witte, K., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of fear appeals: Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ & Beh, 27(5), 591–615.CrossRef Witte, K., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of fear appeals: Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ & Beh, 27(5), 591–615.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Information Processing in the Context of Genetic Risk: Implications for Genetic-Risk Communication
Authors
Holly Etchegary
Colin Perrier
Publication date
01-08-2007
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Genetic Counseling / Issue 4/2007
Print ISSN: 1059-7700
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3599
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10897-006-9082-z

Other articles of this Issue 4/2007

Journal of Genetic Counseling 4/2007 Go to the issue