Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 4/2004

01-06-2004 | Research Article

Is perception of upper body orientation based on the inertia tensor? Normogravity versus microgravity conditions

Authors: N. Gueguen, T. Coyle, C. Craig, R. Bootsma, L. Mouchnino

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 4/2004

Login to get access

Abstract

During lateral leg raising, a synergistic inclination of the supporting leg and trunk in the opposite direction to the leg movement is performed in order to preserve equilibrium. As first hypothesized by Pagano and Turvey (J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 1995, 21:1070–1087), the perception of limb orientation could be based on the orientation of the limb’s inertia tensor. The purpose of this study was thus to explore whether the final upper body orientation (trunk inclination relative to vertical) depends on changes in the trunk inertia tensor. We imposed a loading condition, with total mass of 4 kg added to the subject’s trunk in either a symmetrical or asymmetrical configuration. This changed the orientation of the trunk inertia tensor while keeping the total trunk mass constant. In order to separate any effects of the inertia tensor from the effects of gravitational torque, the experiment was carried out in normo- and microgravity. The results indicated that in normogravity the same final upper body orientation was maintained irrespective of the loading condition. In microgravity, regardless of loading conditions the same (but different from the normogravity) orientation of the upper body was achieved through different joint organizations: two joints (the hip and ankle joints of the supporting leg) in the asymmetrical loading condition, and one (hip) in the symmetrical loading condition. In order to determine whether the different orientations of the inertia tensor were perceived during the movement, the interjoint coordination was quantified by performing a principal components analysis (PCA) on the supporting and moving hips and on the supporting ankle joints. It was expected that different loading conditions would modify the principal component of the PCA. In normogravity, asymmetrical loading decreased the coupling between joints, while in microgravity a strong coupling was preserved whatever the loading condition. It was concluded that the trunk inertia tensor did not play a role during the lateral leg raising task because in spite of the absence of gravitational torque the final upper body orientation and the interjoint coupling were not influenced.
Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Literature
go back to reference Alexandrov A, Frolov AA, Massion J (1994) Voluntary forward bending movement in human: a principal component analysis of axial synergies. In: Taguchi K, Igarashi M, Mori S (eds) Vestibular and neural front. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 345–348 Alexandrov A, Frolov AA, Massion J (1994) Voluntary forward bending movement in human: a principal component analysis of axial synergies. In: Taguchi K, Igarashi M, Mori S (eds) Vestibular and neural front. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 345–348
go back to reference Alexandrov A, Frolov AA, Massion J (1998) Axial synergies during human upper trunk bending. Exp Brain Res 118:210–220PubMed Alexandrov A, Frolov AA, Massion J (1998) Axial synergies during human upper trunk bending. Exp Brain Res 118:210–220PubMed
go back to reference D’Amico M, Ferrigno G (1990) Technique for the evaluation of derivates from noisy biomechanical displacement data using a model-based band width-selection procedure. Med Biol Eng Comput 28:407–415 D’Amico M, Ferrigno G (1990) Technique for the evaluation of derivates from noisy biomechanical displacement data using a model-based band width-selection procedure. Med Biol Eng Comput 28:407–415
go back to reference Ferrigno G, Pedotti A (1985) ELITE: a digital dedicated hardware system for movement analysis via real-time TV signal processing. Trans Biomed Eng 32:943–950PubMed Ferrigno G, Pedotti A (1985) ELITE: a digital dedicated hardware system for movement analysis via real-time TV signal processing. Trans Biomed Eng 32:943–950PubMed
go back to reference Garrett S, Pagano C, Austin G, Turvey T (1998) Spatial and physical frames of reference in positioning a limb. Percept Psychophys 60:1206–1215PubMed Garrett S, Pagano C, Austin G, Turvey T (1998) Spatial and physical frames of reference in positioning a limb. Percept Psychophys 60:1206–1215PubMed
go back to reference Gurfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik Y, Popov KE (1993a) Egocentric references and human spatial orientation in microgravity. A. Perception of complex tactile stimuli. Exp Brain Res 95:339–342PubMed Gurfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik Y, Popov KE (1993a) Egocentric references and human spatial orientation in microgravity. A. Perception of complex tactile stimuli. Exp Brain Res 95:339–342PubMed
go back to reference Gurfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik Y, Popov KE, Lefort L (1993b) Egocentric references and human spatial orientation in microgravity. B. Body-centered coordinates in the task of drawing ellipses with prescribed orientation. Exp Brain Res 95:343–348PubMed Gurfinkel VS, Lestienne F, Levik Y, Popov KE, Lefort L (1993b) Egocentric references and human spatial orientation in microgravity. B. Body-centered coordinates in the task of drawing ellipses with prescribed orientation. Exp Brain Res 95:343–348PubMed
go back to reference Kingma I, Beek PJ, van Dieen JH (2002) The inertia tensor versus static moment and mass in perceiving length and heaviness of hand-wielded rods. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28:180–191CrossRef Kingma I, Beek PJ, van Dieen JH (2002) The inertia tensor versus static moment and mass in perceiving length and heaviness of hand-wielded rods. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 28:180–191CrossRef
go back to reference Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J, Pedotti A (1992) Co-ordination between equilibrium and head–trunk orientation during leg movement: a new strategy built up by training. J Neurophysiol 67:1587–1598PubMed Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J, Pedotti A (1992) Co-ordination between equilibrium and head–trunk orientation during leg movement: a new strategy built up by training. J Neurophysiol 67:1587–1598PubMed
go back to reference Mouchnino L, Cincera M, Fabre J-C, Assaiante C, Amblard B, Pedotti A, Massion J (1996) Is the regulation of the center of mass maintained during leg movement under microgravity conditions? J Neurophysiol 76:1212–1223PubMed Mouchnino L, Cincera M, Fabre J-C, Assaiante C, Amblard B, Pedotti A, Massion J (1996) Is the regulation of the center of mass maintained during leg movement under microgravity conditions? J Neurophysiol 76:1212–1223PubMed
go back to reference Pagano CC, Turvey MT (1995) The inertia tensor as a basis for the perception of limb orientation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1070–1087CrossRefPubMed Pagano CC, Turvey MT (1995) The inertia tensor as a basis for the perception of limb orientation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1070–1087CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Roll R, Gilhodes JC, Roll JP, Popov K, Charade O, Gurfinkel V (1998) Proprioceptive information processing in weightlessness. Exp Brain Res 122:393–402PubMed Roll R, Gilhodes JC, Roll JP, Popov K, Charade O, Gurfinkel V (1998) Proprioceptive information processing in weightlessness. Exp Brain Res 122:393–402PubMed
go back to reference Sainburg RL, Ghez C, Kalakanis D (1999) Inter-segmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. J Neurophysiol 81:1045–1056PubMed Sainburg RL, Ghez C, Kalakanis D (1999) Inter-segmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. J Neurophysiol 81:1045–1056PubMed
go back to reference Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Massion J (1999) Kinematic synergies and equilibrium control during trunk movement under loaded and unloaded conditions. Exp Brain Res 128:517–526CrossRefPubMed Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Massion J (1999) Kinematic synergies and equilibrium control during trunk movement under loaded and unloaded conditions. Exp Brain Res 128:517–526CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Massion J (2000) Kinematic synergy adaptation to microgravity during forward trunk movement. J Neurophysiol 83:453–464PubMed Vernazza-Martin S, Martin N, Massion J (2000) Kinematic synergy adaptation to microgravity during forward trunk movement. J Neurophysiol 83:453–464PubMed
go back to reference Worringham CJ, Stelmach GE (1985) The contribution of gravitational torques to limb position sense. Exp Brain Res 61:38–42PubMed Worringham CJ, Stelmach GE (1985) The contribution of gravitational torques to limb position sense. Exp Brain Res 61:38–42PubMed
go back to reference Zatsiorsky V, Seluyanov V (1983) The mass and inertia characteristics of the main segments of the human body. In: Matsui H, Kobayashi K (eds) Biomechanics VIII-B. International series on biomechanics 4B. Proc 8th Int Congr of Biomechanics, Nagoya, Japan. Human Kinetics Publishers, Champaign, IL, pp 1152–1159 Zatsiorsky V, Seluyanov V (1983) The mass and inertia characteristics of the main segments of the human body. In: Matsui H, Kobayashi K (eds) Biomechanics VIII-B. International series on biomechanics 4B. Proc 8th Int Congr of Biomechanics, Nagoya, Japan. Human Kinetics Publishers, Champaign, IL, pp 1152–1159
Metadata
Title
Is perception of upper body orientation based on the inertia tensor? Normogravity versus microgravity conditions
Authors
N. Gueguen
T. Coyle
C. Craig
R. Bootsma
L. Mouchnino
Publication date
01-06-2004
Publisher
Springer-Verlag
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 4/2004
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1808-7

Other articles of this Issue 4/2004

Experimental Brain Research 4/2004 Go to the issue