Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies 3/2014

01-09-2014 | Original Paper

Expressing Gambling-Related Cognitive Biases in Motor Behaviour: Rolling Dice to Win Prizes

Authors: Matthew S. M. Lim, Henrietta Bowden-Jones, Robert D. Rogers

Published in: Journal of Gambling Studies | Issue 3/2014

Login to get access

Abstract

Cognitive perspectives on gambling propose that biased thinking plays a significant role in sustaining gambling participation and, in vulnerable individuals, gambling problems. One prominent set of cognitive biases include illusions of control involving beliefs that it is possible to influence random gaming events. Sociologists have reported that (some) gamblers believe that it is possible to throw dice in different ways to achieve gaming outcomes (e.g., ‘dice-setting’ in craps). However, experimental demonstrations of these phenomena are lacking. Here, we asked regular gamblers to roll a computer-simulated, but fair, 6 sided die for monetary prizes. Gamblers allowed the die to roll for longer when attempting to win higher value bets, and when attempting to hit high winning numbers. This behaviour was exaggerated in gamblers motivated to keep gambling following the experience of almost-winning in gambling games. These results suggest that gambling cognitive biases find expression in the motor behaviour of rolling dice for monetary prizes, possibly reflecting embodied substrates.
Appendix
Available only for authorised users
Literature
go back to reference Andres, M., Davare, M., Pesenti, M., Olivier, E., & Seron, X. (2004). Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction. NeuroReport, 15(18), 2773–2777.PubMed Andres, M., Davare, M., Pesenti, M., Olivier, E., & Seron, X. (2004). Number magnitude and grip aperture interaction. NeuroReport, 15(18), 2773–2777.PubMed
go back to reference Biner, P. M., Huffman, M. L., Curran, M. A., & Long, K. R. (1998). Illusory control as a function of motivation for a specific outcome in a chance-based situation. Motivation & Emotion, 22(4), 277–291.CrossRef Biner, P. M., Huffman, M. L., Curran, M. A., & Long, K. R. (1998). Illusory control as a function of motivation for a specific outcome in a chance-based situation. Motivation & Emotion, 22(4), 277–291.CrossRef
go back to reference Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motivation and Emotion, 3(4), 381–393.CrossRef Burger, J. M., & Cooper, H. M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motivation and Emotion, 3(4), 381–393.CrossRef
go back to reference Chau, A. W., & Phillips, J. G. (1995). Effects of perceived control upon wagering and attributions in computer blackjack. Journal of General Psychology, 122(3), 253–269.CrossRef Chau, A. W., & Phillips, J. G. (1995). Effects of perceived control upon wagering and attributions in computer blackjack. Journal of General Psychology, 122(3), 253–269.CrossRef
go back to reference Davis, D., Sundahl, I., & Lesbo, M. (2000). Illusory personal control as a determinant of bet size and type in casino craps games. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(6), 1224–1242.CrossRef Davis, D., Sundahl, I., & Lesbo, M. (2000). Illusory personal control as a determinant of bet size and type in casino craps games. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(6), 1224–1242.CrossRef
go back to reference Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRef Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRef
go back to reference Dixon, M. R., & Schreiber, J. E. (2004). Near-miss effects on response latencies and win estimations of slot machine players. The Psychological Record, 54, 335–348. Dixon, M. R., & Schreiber, J. E. (2004). Near-miss effects on response latencies and win estimations of slot machine players. The Psychological Record, 54, 335–348.
go back to reference Dunn, D. S., & Wilson, T. D. (1990). When the stakes are high: A limit to the illusion-of-control effect. Social Cognition, 8(3), 305–323.CrossRef Dunn, D. S., & Wilson, T. D. (1990). When the stakes are high: A limit to the illusion-of-control effect. Social Cognition, 8(3), 305–323.CrossRef
go back to reference Fernandez-Duque, D., & Wifall, T. (2007). Actor/observer asymmetry in risky decision making. Judgment and Decision Making Journal, 2(1), 1–8. Fernandez-Duque, D., & Wifall, T. (2007). Actor/observer asymmetry in risky decision making. Judgment and Decision Making Journal, 2(1), 1–8.
go back to reference First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research. First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research.
go back to reference Fleming, J. H., & Darley, J. M. (1990). The Purposeful-Action Sequence and the Illusion of Control: The Effects of Foreknowledge and Target Involvement on Observers Judgments of Others Control over Random Events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 346–357. doi:10.1177/0146167290162014.CrossRef Fleming, J. H., & Darley, J. M. (1990). The Purposeful-Action Sequence and the Illusion of Control: The Effects of Foreknowledge and Target Involvement on Observers Judgments of Others Control over Random Events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 346–357. doi:10.​1177/​0146167290162014​.CrossRef
go back to reference Friedland, N., Keinan, G., & Regev, Y. (1992). Controlling the uncontrollable: Effects of stress on illusory perceptions of controllability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 923–931.PubMedCrossRef Friedland, N., Keinan, G., & Regev, Y. (1992). Controlling the uncontrollable: Effects of stress on illusory perceptions of controllability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 923–931.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Golin, S., Terrell, F., & Johnson, B. (1977). Depression and the illusion of control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(4), 440–442.PubMedCrossRef Golin, S., Terrell, F., & Johnson, B. (1977). Depression and the illusion of control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(4), 440–442.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Griffiths, M. D. (1990). The cognitive psychology of gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 6(1), 31–42.PubMedCrossRef Griffiths, M. D. (1990). The cognitive psychology of gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 6(1), 31–42.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and Magic. The American Journal of Sociology, 73(3), 316–330.CrossRef Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and Magic. The American Journal of Sociology, 73(3), 316–330.CrossRef
go back to reference Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the “near miss” and the “big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 155–158.PubMedCrossRef Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the “near miss” and the “big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(2), 155–158.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Koehler, J. J., Gibbs, B. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1994). Shattering the illusion of control: Multishot versus single-shot gambles. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7(3), 183–191. doi:10.1002/bdm.3960070304.CrossRef Koehler, J. J., Gibbs, B. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1994). Shattering the illusion of control: Multishot versus single-shot gambles. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7(3), 183–191. doi:10.​1002/​bdm.​3960070304.CrossRef
go back to reference Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ladouceur, R., Gaboury, A., Dumont, M., & Rochette, P. (1988). Gambling: Relationship between the frequency of wins and irrational thinking. Journal of Psychology, 122(4), 409–414.CrossRef Ladouceur, R., Gaboury, A., Dumont, M., & Rochette, P. (1988). Gambling: Relationship between the frequency of wins and irrational thinking. Journal of Psychology, 122(4), 409–414.CrossRef
go back to reference Ladouceur, R., Sylvain, C., Boutin, C., Lachance, S., Doucet, C., Leblond, J., et al. (2001). Cognitive treatment of pathological gambling. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189(11), 774–780.PubMedCrossRef Ladouceur, R., Sylvain, C., Boutin, C., Lachance, S., Doucet, C., Leblond, J., et al. (2001). Cognitive treatment of pathological gambling. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189(11), 774–780.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311–328.CrossRef Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(2), 311–328.CrossRef
go back to reference Martinez, F., Bonnefon, J.-F., & Hoskens, J. (2009). Active involvement, not illusory control, increases risk taking in a gambling game. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1063–1071.CrossRef Martinez, F., Bonnefon, J.-F., & Hoskens, J. (2009). Active involvement, not illusory control, increases risk taking in a gambling game. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1063–1071.CrossRef
go back to reference O’Connor, J., & Dickerson, M. (2003). Definition and measurement of chasing in off-course betting and gaming machine play. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 359–386.PubMedCrossRef O’Connor, J., & Dickerson, M. (2003). Definition and measurement of chasing in off-course betting and gaming machine play. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 359–386.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 110–122. Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 110–122.
go back to reference Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. (2002). Pathological gambling. A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 1009–1061.PubMedCrossRef Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. (2002). Pathological gambling. A comprehensive review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 1009–1061.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.CrossRef Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.CrossRef
go back to reference Rogers, P. (1998). The cognitive psychology of lottery gambling: A theoretical review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(2), 111–134.PubMedCrossRef Rogers, P. (1998). The cognitive psychology of lottery gambling: A theoretical review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(2), 111–134.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Steenbergh, T. A., Meyers, A. W., May, R. K., & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143–149.PubMedCrossRef Steenbergh, T. A., Meyers, A. W., May, R. K., & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143–149.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253–266.PubMedCrossRef Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwood, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253–266.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wardle, H., Moody, A., Spence, S., Orford, J., Volberg, R., Jotangia, D., et al. (2011). British Gambling Prevalence Survey 2010. London: Stationery Office. Wardle, H., Moody, A., Spence, S., Orford, J., Volberg, R., Jotangia, D., et al. (2011). British Gambling Prevalence Survey 2010. London: Stationery Office.
go back to reference Wickwire, E. M., Burke, R. S., Brown, S. A., Parker, J. D., & May, R. K. (2008). Psychometric evaluation of the national opinion research center DSM-IV screen for gambling problems (NODS). American Journal on Addictions, 17(5), 392–395.PubMedCrossRef Wickwire, E. M., Burke, R. S., Brown, S. A., Parker, J. D., & May, R. K. (2008). Psychometric evaluation of the national opinion research center DSM-IV screen for gambling problems (NODS). American Journal on Addictions, 17(5), 392–395.PubMedCrossRef
Metadata
Title
Expressing Gambling-Related Cognitive Biases in Motor Behaviour: Rolling Dice to Win Prizes
Authors
Matthew S. M. Lim
Henrietta Bowden-Jones
Robert D. Rogers
Publication date
01-09-2014
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Journal of Gambling Studies / Issue 3/2014
Electronic ISSN: 1573-3602
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-013-9381-x

Other articles of this Issue 3/2014

Journal of Gambling Studies 3/2014 Go to the issue