Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Experimental Brain Research 2/2012

01-01-2012 | Research Article

Directional remapping in tactile inter-finger apparent motion: a motion aftereffect study

Authors: Scinob Kuroki, Junji Watanabe, Kunihiko Mabuchi, Susumu Tachi, Shin’ya Nishida

Published in: Experimental Brain Research | Issue 2/2012

Login to get access

Abstract

Tactile motion provides critical information for perception and manipulation of objects in touch. Perceived directions of tactile motion are primarily defined in the environmental coordinate, which means they change drastically with body posture even when the same skin sensors are stimulated. Despite the ecological importance of this perceptual constancy, the sensory processing underlying tactile directional remapping remains poorly understood. The present study psychophysically investigated the mechanisms underlying directional remapping in human tactile motion processing by examining whether finger posture modulates the direction of the tactile motion aftereffect (MAE) induced by inter-finger apparent motions. We introduced conflicts in the adaptation direction between somatotopic and environmental spaces by having participants change their finger posture between adaptation and test phases. In a critical condition, they touched stimulators with crossed index and middle fingers during adaptation but with uncrossed fingers during tests. Since the adaptation effect was incongruent between the somatotopic and environmental spaces, the direction of the MAE reflects the coordinate of tactile motion processing. The results demonstrated that the tactile MAE was induced in accordance with the motion direction determined by the environmental rather than the somatotopic space. In addition, it was found that though the physical adaptation of the test fingers was not changed, the tactile MAE disappeared when the adaptation stimuli were vertically aligned or when subjective motion perception was suppressed during adaptation. We also found that the tactile MAE, measured with our procedure, did not transfer across different hands, which implies that the observed MAEs mainly reflect neural adaptations occurring within sensor-specific, tactile-specific processing. The present findings provide a novel behavioral method to analyze the neural representation for directional remapping of tactile motion within tactile sensory processing in the human brain.
Footnotes
1
In vision, MAE occurs at multiple levels of processing (Nishida and Ashida 2000; Nishida and Sato 1995), and attentive tracking alone can induce a high-level MAE even with no directional biases in low-level motion adaptation signals (Culham et al. 2010). It remains unclear whether attentive tracking can also induce a tactile MAE in a similar way. To minimize the potential influence of attentive tracking on our tactile MAE, we instructed the participants not to track the adapting motion. Although it might be difficult for this procedure to completely prevent some participants from occasionally tracking the adapting stimuli, the lack of the MAE in the contralateral condition in Experiment 2, which examined a high-level MAE, might indicate that the effect of attentive tracking was negligible in our case, either because attentive tracking has no effect at least on the type of tactile MAE we measured, or attentive tracking during adaptation was effectively reduced by the instruction. Further investigations are needed on the relationship between attention and tactile MAEs.
 
2
The results remained the same even when the author’s data were excluded. The MAE magnitude was significantly different from zero in the uncrossed and crossed conditions [t(8) = 10.11, P < 0.001 and t(8) = 9.70, P < 0.001, respectively], but not in the vertically aligned (in Experiment 1), crossed + index (in Experiment 2), and contralateral (in Experiment 2) conditions [t(8) = 0.82, P > 0.1, ns; t(8) = 0.47, P > 0.1, ns; and t(8) = 0.20, P > 0.1, ns, respectively].
 
3
One might suspect that the reason why the average MAE magnitude of the vertically aligned condition was close to zero was not because the MAE magnitude was small, but because the MAE direction of MAE was inconsistent across participants. In disagreement with this possibility, however, the adaptation effect was significantly smaller for the vertically aligned condition than for the uncrossed condition even when the absolute (unsigned) shift magnitude was compared [t(9) = 5.89, P < 0.001, by paired t test].
 
4
Although we used the crossed posture during adaptation and the uncrossed posture in the test, it is possible in theory to test directional remapping by using the uncrossed posture during adaptation and the crossed posture in the test. In a preliminary experiment, we tried this finger condition, but the results did not show a decisive trend. This is at least in part because the crossed finger posture made direction judgments less accurate (Benedetti 1985; Sekine and Mogi 2009 see also subsidiary experiments), as hand crossing does (Azañón and Soto-Faraco 2008; Yamamoto and Kitazawa 2001; Shore et al. 2002). Psychometric functions became shallow and distorted (Yamamoto and Kitazawa 2001), and then, the estimation of the MAE from the shift of psychometric functions became less reliable.
 
5
Since motion intensity rating was close to “1” rather than “2” even for normal finger posture, one might consider that the apparent motions we presented were not strong. However, this may not be necessarily so, because the criterion to choose “2” was arbitrarily set by each participant. In addition, given a small variation in the magnitude of motion impression across trials in our experiment, some participants might use “1” to indicate the standard intensity of apparent motion presented in the session. Although the rating was not high, it is very unlikely that the apparent motion was not perceived at all for the first three conditions, since their motion intensity ratings were significantly higher than that for the crossed + index condition.
 
Literature
go back to reference Azañón E, Soto-Faraco S (2008) Changing reference frames during the encoding of tactile events. Curr Biol 18:1044–1049PubMedCrossRef Azañón E, Soto-Faraco S (2008) Changing reference frames during the encoding of tactile events. Curr Biol 18:1044–1049PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Azañón E, Longo MR, Soto-Faraco S, Haggard P (2010) The posterior parietal cortex remaps touch into external space. Curr Biol 20:1304–1309PubMedCrossRef Azañón E, Longo MR, Soto-Faraco S, Haggard P (2010) The posterior parietal cortex remaps touch into external space. Curr Biol 20:1304–1309PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Benedetti F (1985) Processing of tactile spatial information with crossed fingers. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:517–525PubMedCrossRef Benedetti F (1985) Processing of tactile spatial information with crossed fingers. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:517–525PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bensmaїa SJ, Leung YY, Hsiao SS, Johnson KO (2005) Vibratory adaptation of cutaneous mechanoreceptive afferents. J Neurophysiol 94:3023–3036CrossRef Bensmaїa SJ, Leung YY, Hsiao SS, Johnson KO (2005) Vibratory adaptation of cutaneous mechanoreceptive afferents. J Neurophysiol 94:3023–3036CrossRef
go back to reference Bensmaїa SJ, Denchev PV, Dammann JF, Craig JC, Hsiao SS (2008) The representation of stimulus orientation in the early stages of somatosensory processing. J Neurosci 28:776–786CrossRef Bensmaїa SJ, Denchev PV, Dammann JF, Craig JC, Hsiao SS (2008) The representation of stimulus orientation in the early stages of somatosensory processing. J Neurosci 28:776–786CrossRef
go back to reference Blake R, Tadin D, Sobel KV, Raissian TA, Chong SC (2006) Strength of early visual adaptation depends on visual awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:4783–4788PubMedCrossRef Blake R, Tadin D, Sobel KV, Raissian TA, Chong SC (2006) Strength of early visual adaptation depends on visual awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:4783–4788PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chung S, Li X, Nelson SB (2002) Short-term depression at thalamocortical synapses contributes to rapid adaptation of cortical sensory responses in vivo. Neuron 34:437–446PubMedCrossRef Chung S, Li X, Nelson SB (2002) Short-term depression at thalamocortical synapses contributes to rapid adaptation of cortical sensory responses in vivo. Neuron 34:437–446PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Costanzo RM, Gardner EP (1980) A quantitative analysis of responses of direction-sensitive neurons in somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. J Neurophysiol 43:1319–1341PubMed Costanzo RM, Gardner EP (1980) A quantitative analysis of responses of direction-sensitive neurons in somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. J Neurophysiol 43:1319–1341PubMed
go back to reference Craig JC (2003) The effect of hand position and pattern motion on temporal order judgments. Percept Psychophys 65:779–788PubMedCrossRef Craig JC (2003) The effect of hand position and pattern motion on temporal order judgments. Percept Psychophys 65:779–788PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Culham JC, Verstraten FA, Ashida H, Cavanagh P (2010) Independent aftereffects of attention and motion. Neuron 28:607–615CrossRef Culham JC, Verstraten FA, Ashida H, Cavanagh P (2010) Independent aftereffects of attention and motion. Neuron 28:607–615CrossRef
go back to reference de Lafuente V, Romo R (2005) Neuronal correlates of subjective sensory experience. Nat Neurosci 8:1698–1703PubMedCrossRef de Lafuente V, Romo R (2005) Neuronal correlates of subjective sensory experience. Nat Neurosci 8:1698–1703PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Driver J, Grossenbacher PG (1996) Multimodal spatial constraints on tactile selective attention. In: Innui T, McClelland J (eds) Attention and performance. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 209–235 Driver J, Grossenbacher PG (1996) Multimodal spatial constraints on tactile selective attention. In: Innui T, McClelland J (eds) Attention and performance. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 209–235
go back to reference Friedman RM, Chen LM, Roe AW (2004) Modality maps within primate somatosensory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:12724–12729PubMedCrossRef Friedman RM, Chen LM, Roe AW (2004) Modality maps within primate somatosensory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:12724–12729PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Graziano MS, Yap GS, Gross CG (1994) Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 266:1054–1057PubMedCrossRef Graziano MS, Yap GS, Gross CG (1994) Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 266:1054–1057PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hagen MC, Franzen O, McGlone F, Essick G, Dancer C, Pardo JV (2002) Tactile motion activates the human middle temporal/V5 (MT/V5) complex. Eur J Neurosci 16:957–964PubMedCrossRef Hagen MC, Franzen O, McGlone F, Essick G, Dancer C, Pardo JV (2002) Tactile motion activates the human middle temporal/V5 (MT/V5) complex. Eur J Neurosci 16:957–964PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Iwamura Y, Tanaka M, Sakamoto M, Hikosaka O (1983) Converging patterns of finger representation and complex response properties of neurons in area 1 of the first somatosensory cortex of the conscious monkey. Exp Brain Res 51:327–337 Iwamura Y, Tanaka M, Sakamoto M, Hikosaka O (1983) Converging patterns of finger representation and complex response properties of neurons in area 1 of the first somatosensory cortex of the conscious monkey. Exp Brain Res 51:327–337
go back to reference Iwamura Y, Tanaka M, Sakamoto M, Hikosaka O (1993) Rostrocaudal gradients in the neuronal receptive field complexity in the finger region of the alert monkey’s postcentral gyrus. Exp Brain Res 92:360–368PubMedCrossRef Iwamura Y, Tanaka M, Sakamoto M, Hikosaka O (1993) Rostrocaudal gradients in the neuronal receptive field complexity in the finger region of the alert monkey’s postcentral gyrus. Exp Brain Res 92:360–368PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kaas JH, Nelson RJ, Sur M, Lin CS, Merzenich MM (1979) Multiple representations of the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Science 204:521–523PubMedCrossRef Kaas JH, Nelson RJ, Sur M, Lin CS, Merzenich MM (1979) Multiple representations of the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Science 204:521–523PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kirman JH (1974) Tactile apparent movement: the effects of interstimulus onset interval and stimulus duration. Percept Psychophys 15:1–6CrossRef Kirman JH (1974) Tactile apparent movement: the effects of interstimulus onset interval and stimulus duration. Percept Psychophys 15:1–6CrossRef
go back to reference Konkle T, Wang Q, Hayward V, Moore CI (2009) Motion aftereffects transfer between touch and vision. Curr Biol 12:745–750CrossRef Konkle T, Wang Q, Hayward V, Moore CI (2009) Motion aftereffects transfer between touch and vision. Curr Biol 12:745–750CrossRef
go back to reference Kuroki S, Watanabe J, Kawakami N, Tachi S, Nishida S (2010) Somatotopic dominance in tactile temporal processing. Exp Brain Res 203:51–62PubMedCrossRef Kuroki S, Watanabe J, Kawakami N, Tachi S, Nishida S (2010) Somatotopic dominance in tactile temporal processing. Exp Brain Res 203:51–62PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kurth R, Villringer K, Curio G, Wolf KJ, Krause T, Repenthin J, Schwiemann J, Deuchert M, Villringer A (2000) fMRI shows multiple somatotopic digit representations in human primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroreport 11:1487–1491PubMedCrossRef Kurth R, Villringer K, Curio G, Wolf KJ, Krause T, Repenthin J, Schwiemann J, Deuchert M, Villringer A (2000) fMRI shows multiple somatotopic digit representations in human primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroreport 11:1487–1491PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lehmkuhle SW, Fox R (1975) Effect of binocular rivalry suppression on the motion aftereffect. Vis Res 15:855–859PubMedCrossRef Lehmkuhle SW, Fox R (1975) Effect of binocular rivalry suppression on the motion aftereffect. Vis Res 15:855–859PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lerner EA, Craig JC (2002) The prevalence of tactile motion aftereffects. Somatosens Mot Res 19:24–29PubMedCrossRef Lerner EA, Craig JC (2002) The prevalence of tactile motion aftereffects. Somatosens Mot Res 19:24–29PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maruya K, Watanabe H, Watanabe M (2008) Adaptation to invisible motion results in low-level but not high-level aftereffects. J Vis 8:1–11PubMedCrossRef Maruya K, Watanabe H, Watanabe M (2008) Adaptation to invisible motion results in low-level but not high-level aftereffects. J Vis 8:1–11PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mather G, Verstraten FAJ, Anstis SM (1998) The motion aftereffect: a modern perspective. MIT Press, Cambridge Mather G, Verstraten FAJ, Anstis SM (1998) The motion aftereffect: a modern perspective. MIT Press, Cambridge
go back to reference Nishida S, Ashida H (2000) A hierarchical structure of motion system revealed by interocular transfer of flicker motion aftereffects. Vis Res 40:265–278PubMedCrossRef Nishida S, Ashida H (2000) A hierarchical structure of motion system revealed by interocular transfer of flicker motion aftereffects. Vis Res 40:265–278PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nishida S, Sato T (1995) Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing. Vis Res 35:477–490PubMedCrossRef Nishida S, Sato T (1995) Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing. Vis Res 35:477–490PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference O’Mara S, Rowe MJ, Tarvin RP (1988) Neural mechanisms in vibrotactile adaptation. J Neurophysiol 59:607–622PubMed O’Mara S, Rowe MJ, Tarvin RP (1988) Neural mechanisms in vibrotactile adaptation. J Neurophysiol 59:607–622PubMed
go back to reference Pei YC, Hsiao SS, Craing JC, Bensmaїa SJ (2010) Shape invariant coding of motion direction in somatosensory cortex. PLoS Biol 8:e1000305PubMedCrossRef Pei YC, Hsiao SS, Craing JC, Bensmaїa SJ (2010) Shape invariant coding of motion direction in somatosensory cortex. PLoS Biol 8:e1000305PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Pei YC, Hsiao SS, Craing JC, Bensmaїa SJ (2011) Neural mechanisms of tactile motion integration in somatosensory cortex. Neuron 69:536–547PubMedCrossRef Pei YC, Hsiao SS, Craing JC, Bensmaїa SJ (2011) Neural mechanisms of tactile motion integration in somatosensory cortex. Neuron 69:536–547PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Planetta PJ, Servos P (2008) The tactile motion aftereffect revisited. Somatosens Mot Res 25:93–99PubMedCrossRef Planetta PJ, Servos P (2008) The tactile motion aftereffect revisited. Somatosens Mot Res 25:93–99PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Planetta PJ, Servos P (2010) Site of stimulation effects on the prevalence of the tactile motion aftereffect. Exp Brain Res 202:377–383PubMedCrossRef Planetta PJ, Servos P (2010) Site of stimulation effects on the prevalence of the tactile motion aftereffect. Exp Brain Res 202:377–383PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rees G, Frith CD, Lavie N (1997) Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science 278:1616–1619PubMedCrossRef Rees G, Frith CD, Lavie N (1997) Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. Science 278:1616–1619PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rinker MA, Craig JC (1994) The effect of spatial orientation on the perception of moving tactile stimuli. Percept Psycophys 56:356–362CrossRef Rinker MA, Craig JC (1994) The effect of spatial orientation on the perception of moving tactile stimuli. Percept Psycophys 56:356–362CrossRef
go back to reference Ruiz S, Crespo P, Romo R (1995) Representation of moving tactile stimuli in the somatic sensory cortex of awake monkeys. J Neurophysiol 73:525–537PubMed Ruiz S, Crespo P, Romo R (1995) Representation of moving tactile stimuli in the somatic sensory cortex of awake monkeys. J Neurophysiol 73:525–537PubMed
go back to reference Sakata H, Takaoka Y, Kawarasaki A, Shibutani H (1973) Somatosensory properties of neurons in the superior parietal cortex (area 5) of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 64:85–102PubMedCrossRef Sakata H, Takaoka Y, Kawarasaki A, Shibutani H (1973) Somatosensory properties of neurons in the superior parietal cortex (area 5) of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 64:85–102PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sanabria D, Soto-Faraco S, Spence C (2005) Spatiotemporal interactions between audition and touch depend on hand posture. Exp Brain Res 165:505–514PubMedCrossRef Sanabria D, Soto-Faraco S, Spence C (2005) Spatiotemporal interactions between audition and touch depend on hand posture. Exp Brain Res 165:505–514PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sekine T, Mogi K (2009) Distinct neural processes of bodily awareness in crossed fingers illusion. Neuroreport 20:467–472PubMedCrossRef Sekine T, Mogi K (2009) Distinct neural processes of bodily awareness in crossed fingers illusion. Neuroreport 20:467–472PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sherrick CE (1968) Bilateral apparent haptic movement. Percept Psychophys 4:159–160CrossRef Sherrick CE (1968) Bilateral apparent haptic movement. Percept Psychophys 4:159–160CrossRef
go back to reference Sherrick CE, Rogers R (1966) Apparent haptic movement. Percept Psycophys 1:175–180 Sherrick CE, Rogers R (1966) Apparent haptic movement. Percept Psycophys 1:175–180
go back to reference Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C (2002) Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14:153–163PubMedCrossRef Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C (2002) Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14:153–163PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Simons SB, Chiu J, Favorov OV, Whitsel BL, Tommerdahl M (2007) Duration-dependent response of SI to vibrotactile stimulation in squirrel monkey. J Neurophysiol 97:2121–2129PubMedCrossRef Simons SB, Chiu J, Favorov OV, Whitsel BL, Tommerdahl M (2007) Duration-dependent response of SI to vibrotactile stimulation in squirrel monkey. J Neurophysiol 97:2121–2129PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soto-Faraco S, Ronald A, Spence C (2004a) Tactile selective attention and body posture: assessing the multisensory contributions of vision and proprioception. Percept Psychophys 66:1077–1094PubMedCrossRef Soto-Faraco S, Ronald A, Spence C (2004a) Tactile selective attention and body posture: assessing the multisensory contributions of vision and proprioception. Percept Psychophys 66:1077–1094PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Soto-Faraco S, Spence C, Kingstone A (2004b) Congruency effects between auditory and tactile motion: extending the phenomenon of cross-modal dynamic capture. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:208–217PubMedCrossRef Soto-Faraco S, Spence C, Kingstone A (2004b) Congruency effects between auditory and tactile motion: extending the phenomenon of cross-modal dynamic capture. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4:208–217PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Summers IR, Francis ST, Bowtell RW, McGlone FP, Clemence M (2009) A functional-magnetic-resonance-imaging investigation of cortical activation from moving vibrotactile stimuli on the fingertip. J Acoust Soc Am 125:1033–1039PubMedCrossRef Summers IR, Francis ST, Bowtell RW, McGlone FP, Clemence M (2009) A functional-magnetic-resonance-imaging investigation of cortical activation from moving vibrotactile stimuli on the fingertip. J Acoust Soc Am 125:1033–1039PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thalman WA (1922) The after-effect of movement in the sense of touch. Am J Psychol 33:268–276CrossRef Thalman WA (1922) The after-effect of movement in the sense of touch. Am J Psychol 33:268–276CrossRef
go back to reference Warren S, Hamalainen HA, Gardner EP (1986) Objective classification of motion- and direction-sensitive neurons in primary somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. J Neurophysiol 56:598–622PubMed Warren S, Hamalainen HA, Gardner EP (1986) Objective classification of motion- and direction-sensitive neurons in primary somatosensory cortex of awake monkeys. J Neurophysiol 56:598–622PubMed
go back to reference Watanabe J, Hayashi S, Kajimoto H, Tachi S, Nishida S (2007) Tactile motion aftereffects produced by appropriate presentation for mechanoreceptors. Exp Brain Res 180:577–582PubMedCrossRef Watanabe J, Hayashi S, Kajimoto H, Tachi S, Nishida S (2007) Tactile motion aftereffects produced by appropriate presentation for mechanoreceptors. Exp Brain Res 180:577–582PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Whitney D, Bressler DW (2007) Second-order motion without awareness: passive adaptation to second-order motion produces a motion aftereffect. Vis Res 47:569–579PubMedCrossRef Whitney D, Bressler DW (2007) Second-order motion without awareness: passive adaptation to second-order motion produces a motion aftereffect. Vis Res 47:569–579PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wiesenfelder H, Blake R (1990) The neural site of binocular rivalry relative to the analysis of motion in the human visual system. J Neurosci 10:3880–3888PubMed Wiesenfelder H, Blake R (1990) The neural site of binocular rivalry relative to the analysis of motion in the human visual system. J Neurosci 10:3880–3888PubMed
go back to reference Wohlgemuth A (1911) On the after-effect of seen movement. Br J Psychol 1:1–117 Wohlgemuth A (1911) On the after-effect of seen movement. Br J Psychol 1:1–117
go back to reference Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4:759–765PubMedCrossRef Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4:759–765PubMedCrossRef
Metadata
Title
Directional remapping in tactile inter-finger apparent motion: a motion aftereffect study
Authors
Scinob Kuroki
Junji Watanabe
Kunihiko Mabuchi
Susumu Tachi
Shin’ya Nishida
Publication date
01-01-2012
Publisher
Springer-Verlag
Published in
Experimental Brain Research / Issue 2/2012
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Electronic ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2936-0

Other articles of this Issue 2/2012

Experimental Brain Research 2/2012 Go to the issue