Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 6/2012

Open Access 01-12-2012 | Original Article

Cognitive Distortions and Self-Regulatory Personality Traits Associated with Proactive and Reactive Aggression in Early Adolescence

Authors: Sophieke Koolen, Astrid Poorthuis, Marcel A. G. van Aken

Published in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Issue 6/2012

Login to get access

Abstract

This study investigated mechanisms behind proactive and reactive aggression, by examining whether four types of self-serving cognitive distortions and the personality traits agreeableness and conscientiousness differently predicted proactive and reactive aggression. Self-report questionnaires and a peer nominations method were administered to 173 sixth grade children (age 10–13) of regular elementary schools in the Netherlands. Negative binomial regression analyses showed that proactive aggression was predicted by self-centered and disagreeable tendencies, whereas reactive aggression was predicted by the misattribution of blame to others and the self-regulatory aspects of agreeableness and conscientiousness. Findings emphasize the need to differentiate proactive and reactive aggression in order to accurately predict, prevent and treat aggressive behaviors in childhood.
Footnotes
1
The questionnaire contained an additional scale to measure social desirability. The scale consisted of seven items, for instance ‘I have sometimes said something bad about a friend’ (Cronbach’s α = .75), where higher scores indicated lower levels of social desirability. To account for possible effects of social desirability on the results, two data files were composed; one with all participants and one without participants with a score on the anomalous response scale of the HIT-Q below the cut-off value 2.75. All analyses were conducted with both data files. Since no differences in results were found, it was decided to use the complete data file, in order to obtain optimal power.
 
2
Although a Poisson regression is the standard method to model rate data, the present data showed overdispersion, indicating that the observed variances were larger than the means (M = 5.08, σ² = 72.88 for proactive aggression and M = .50, σ² = 1.03 for reactive aggression). Because Poisson’s assumption of equidispersion (the variance being equal to its mean) was violated, a negative binomial model was chosen, which models overdispersed Poisson data. Since the possibility of being nominated depended upon of number of children in the class, the differences in class size (differences in the exposure to the nominations) needed to be controlled for. In negative binomial regression this is handled as an offset (the natural logarithm of class size-1). The offset was included as a predictor variable in the model, but was differentiated from other variables by being analyzed as a constant (forced to have a coefficient 1) and therefore being independent of scores of other predictor variables (Agresti 2002).
 
Literature
go back to reference Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament, development and the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 186–207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament, development and the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 186–207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral development: Integrating social information processing and moral domain models. Child Development, 75(4), 987–1002.PubMedCrossRef Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral development: Integrating social information processing and moral domain models. Child Development, 75(4), 987–1002.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Arsenio, W. F., Adams, E., & Gold, J. (2009). Social information processing, moral reasoning and emotion attributions: Relations with adolescents’ reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 80(6), 1739–1755.PubMedCrossRef Arsenio, W. F., Adams, E., & Gold, J. (2009). Social information processing, moral reasoning and emotion attributions: Relations with adolescents’ reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 80(6), 1739–1755.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Personality-relationship transaction in adolescence: Core versus surface personality characteristics. Journal of Personality, 71, 629–666.PubMedCrossRef Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Personality-relationship transaction in adolescence: Core versus surface personality characteristics. Journal of Personality, 71, 629–666.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barriga, A. Q., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Measuring cognitive distortions in antisocial youth: Development and preliminary validation of the ‘how I think’ questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 333–343.CrossRef Barriga, A. Q., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Measuring cognitive distortions in antisocial youth: Development and preliminary validation of the ‘how I think’ questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 333–343.CrossRef
go back to reference Barriga, A. Q., Hawkins, M. A., & Camelia, C. R. T. (2008). Specificity of cognitive distortions to antisocial behaviors. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 18, 104–116.PubMedCrossRef Barriga, A. Q., Hawkins, M. A., & Camelia, C. R. T. (2008). Specificity of cognitive distortions to antisocial behaviors. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 18, 104–116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73.PubMedCrossRef Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 466–480.CrossRef Card, N. A., & Little, T. D. (2006). Proactive and reactive aggression in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis of differential relations with psychosocial adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 466–480.CrossRef
go back to reference Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. Damen & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 300–365). New York: Wiley. Caspi, A., & Shiner, R. L. (2006). Personality development. In W. Damen & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 300–365). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.CrossRef Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.CrossRef
go back to reference Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social-information processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002.PubMedCrossRef Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social-information processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Denissen, J. J. A., Geenen, R., Van Aken, M. A. G., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). Development and validation of a Dutch translation of the big five inventory (BFI). Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 152–157.PubMedCrossRef Denissen, J. J. A., Geenen, R., Van Aken, M. A. G., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). Development and validation of a Dutch translation of the big five inventory (BFI). Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 152–157.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146–1158.PubMedCrossRef Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146–1158.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dodge, K. A., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Returning to roots: On social information processing and moral development. Child Development, 75(4), 1003–1008.PubMedCrossRef Dodge, K. A., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Returning to roots: On social information processing and moral development. Child Development, 75(4), 1003–1008.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fite, J. E., Goodnight, J. A., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2008). Adolescent aggression and social cognition in the context of personality: Impulsivity as a moderator of predictions from social information processing. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 511–520.PubMedCrossRef Fite, J. E., Goodnight, J. A., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2008). Adolescent aggression and social cognition in the context of personality: Impulsivity as a moderator of predictions from social information processing. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 511–520.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Hair, E. C. (1996). Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(4), 820–835.PubMedCrossRef Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Hair, E. C. (1996). Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(4), 820–835.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information-processing model for the development of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 13–24.CrossRef Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information-processing model for the development of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 13–24.CrossRef
go back to reference Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Guerra, N. G., & Crawshaw, V. B. (1994). Measuring children’s aggression with teachers’ predictions of peer-nomination. Psychological Assessment, 6, 329–336.CrossRef Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Guerra, N. G., & Crawshaw, V. B. (1994). Measuring children’s aggression with teachers’ predictions of peer-nomination. Psychological Assessment, 6, 329–336.CrossRef
go back to reference Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children’s normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 408–419.PubMedCrossRef Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children’s normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 408–419.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Rosselli, M., Workman, K. A., Santisi, M., Rios, J. D., & Bojan, D. (2002). Agreeableness, conscientiousness, and effortful control processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 476–489.CrossRef Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Rosselli, M., Workman, K. A., Santisi, M., Rios, J. D., & Bojan, D. (2002). Agreeableness, conscientiousness, and effortful control processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 476–489.CrossRef
go back to reference Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Knack, J. N., Waldrip, A. M., & Campbell, S. D. (2007). Do big five personality traits associated with self-control influence the regulation of anger and aggression? Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 403–424.CrossRef Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Knack, J. N., Waldrip, A. M., & Campbell, S. D. (2007). Do big five personality traits associated with self-control influence the regulation of anger and aggression? Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 403–424.CrossRef
go back to reference Liau, A. K., Barriga, A. Q., & Gibbs, J. C. (1998). Relations between self-serving cognitive distortions and overt versus covert antisocial behavior in adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 335–346.CrossRef Liau, A. K., Barriga, A. Q., & Gibbs, J. C. (1998). Relations between self-serving cognitive distortions and overt versus covert antisocial behavior in adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 335–346.CrossRef
go back to reference Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. B. (1985). Empirical evidence for overt and covert patterns of antisocial conduct problems: A metaanalysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13(2), 337–352.PubMedCrossRef Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. B. (1985). Empirical evidence for overt and covert patterns of antisocial conduct problems: A metaanalysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13(2), 337–352.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marsee, M. A., & Frick, P. J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive and emotional correlates to proactive and reactive aggression in a sample of detained girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 969–981.PubMedCrossRef Marsee, M. A., & Frick, P. J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive and emotional correlates to proactive and reactive aggression in a sample of detained girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 969–981.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Does quick to blame mean quick to anger? The role of agreeableness in dissociating blame and anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 856–867.PubMedCrossRef Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Does quick to blame mean quick to anger? The role of agreeableness in dissociating blame and anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 856–867.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nas, C. N., Brugman, D., & Koops, W. (2008). Measuring self-serving cognitive distortions with the ‘how I think’ questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24(3), 181–189. Nas, C. N., Brugman, D., & Koops, W. (2008). Measuring self-serving cognitive distortions with the ‘how I think’ questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24(3), 181–189.
go back to reference Palmer, E. J. (2003). An overview of the relationship between moral reasoning and offending. Australian Psychologist, 38(3), 165–174.CrossRef Palmer, E. J. (2003). An overview of the relationship between moral reasoning and offending. Australian Psychologist, 38(3), 165–174.CrossRef
go back to reference Palmer, E. J. (2005). The relationship between moral reasoning and aggression, and the implications for practice. Psychology Crime and Law, 11(4), 353–361.CrossRef Palmer, E. J. (2005). The relationship between moral reasoning and aggression, and the implications for practice. Psychology Crime and Law, 11(4), 353–361.CrossRef
go back to reference Peets, K., Hodges, E. V. E., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). Actualization of social cognitions into aggressive behavior toward disliked targets. Social Development, 20(2), 233–250.CrossRef Peets, K., Hodges, E. V. E., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). Actualization of social cognitions into aggressive behavior toward disliked targets. Social Development, 20(2), 233–250.CrossRef
go back to reference Polman, H., Orobio de Castro, B., Koops, W., Van Boxtel, H. W., & Merk, W. (2007). Meta analysis of the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 522–535.PubMedCrossRef Polman, H., Orobio de Castro, B., Koops, W., Van Boxtel, H. W., & Merk, W. (2007). Meta analysis of the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 522–535.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Polman, H., Orobio de Castro, B., Thomaes, S. E., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2008). New directions in measuring proactive and reactive aggression: Validation of a teacher questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 183–193.CrossRef Polman, H., Orobio de Castro, B., Thomaes, S. E., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2008). New directions in measuring proactive and reactive aggression: Validation of a teacher questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 183–193.CrossRef
go back to reference Sim, T. N., & Koh, S. F. (2003). A domain conceptualization of adolescent susceptibility to peer pressure. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 57–80.CrossRef Sim, T. N., & Koh, S. F. (2003). A domain conceptualization of adolescent susceptibility to peer pressure. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 57–80.CrossRef
go back to reference Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A., & Simons, R. F. (2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in delinquent adolescents: Relations to aggression outcome expectancies. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(1), 86–93.PubMedCrossRef Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A., & Simons, R. F. (2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in delinquent adolescents: Relations to aggression outcome expectancies. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(1), 86–93.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
go back to reference van der Velden, F., Brugman, D., Boom, J., & Koops, W. (2010). Moral cognitive processes explaining antisocial behavior in young adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34(4), 292–301.CrossRef van der Velden, F., Brugman, D., Boom, J., & Koops, W. (2010). Moral cognitive processes explaining antisocial behavior in young adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34(4), 292–301.CrossRef
go back to reference Vitaro, F., Gendreau, P. L., Tremblay, R. E., & Oligny, P. (1998). Reactive and proactive aggression differentially predict later conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 377–385.PubMedCrossRef Vitaro, F., Gendreau, P. L., Tremblay, R. E., & Oligny, P. (1998). Reactive and proactive aggression differentially predict later conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 377–385.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Weiss, B., Harris, V., & Catron, T. (2002). Development and validation of the peer-report measure of internalizing and externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 258–294.CrossRef Weiss, B., Harris, V., & Catron, T. (2002). Development and validation of the peer-report measure of internalizing and externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 258–294.CrossRef
Metadata
Title
Cognitive Distortions and Self-Regulatory Personality Traits Associated with Proactive and Reactive Aggression in Early Adolescence
Authors
Sophieke Koolen
Astrid Poorthuis
Marcel A. G. van Aken
Publication date
01-12-2012
Publisher
Springer US
Published in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Issue 6/2012
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Electronic ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9407-6

Other articles of this Issue 6/2012

Cognitive Therapy and Research 6/2012 Go to the issue