Skip to main content
Top
Published in: Prevention Science 2/2007

01-06-2007 | Original Paper

A Biosocial-Affect Model of Adolescent Sensation Seeking: The Role of Affect Evaluation and Peer-Group Influence in Adolescent Drug Use

Authors: Daniel Romer, Michael Hennessy

Published in: Prevention Science | Issue 2/2007

Login to get access

Abstract

Adolescence is a period of heightened experimentation with risky behavior. Models of brain development suggest that this phenomenon is partly the result of increased adolescent sensation seeking unaccompanied by maturation in ability to evaluate risks. We test an alternative biosocial-affect model in which favorable affect attached to behavior leads to discounting of risks. Although the model applies to both adolescents and adults, it predicts that the surge in sensation seeking during adolescence increases affective attraction to risky behavior, reduces perceived risk of the behavior, and results in peer-group reinforcement of these effects. We estimated models that included these influences for three drugs (tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana) in a national sample of youth ages 14 to 22. Consistent with brain maturation models, sensation seeking rose during the age period under study with girls peaking earlier than boys. Nevertheless, independent of age or gender, the biosocial-affect model explained the relation between sensation seeking and drug use. The findings indicate that although adolescents recognize the risks of drug use, they are subject to both biological and social influences that encourage risk taking. Implications for the prevention of risky adolescent behavior are discussed.
Footnotes
1
We did not explicitly test the measurement models for the affect heuristic and peer attraction factors. However, the high loadings of the variables that defined these factors, and the satisfactory goodness of fit indices for the entire models (shown in the Results) support the adequacy of the measurement assumptions for these factors.
 
2
Our interest in separating the effects of sensation seeking on drug use into affect evaluation and, peer attraction was to compare the relative sizes of these mediational paths, not to define confidence intervals for them. Hence, we do not conduct additional tests of significance for these components.
 
Literature
go back to reference Arnett, J. J. (1992). Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective. Developmental Review, 12, 339–373.CrossRef Arnett, J. J. (1992). Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective. Developmental Review, 12, 339–373.CrossRef
go back to reference Audrain-Mcgovern, J., Rodriguez, D., & Moss, H. B. (2003). Smoking progression and physical activity. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention , 12, 1121–1129. Audrain-Mcgovern, J., Rodriguez, D., & Moss, H. B. (2003). Smoking progression and physical activity. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention , 12, 1121–1129.
go back to reference Bachman, J. G., Johnston, L. D., & O’Malley, P. M. (1990). Explaining the recent decline in cocaine use among young adults: Further evidence that perceived risks and disapproval lead to reduced drug use. Journal of Health and Social Behavior , 31, 173–184.PubMedCrossRef Bachman, J. G., Johnston, L. D., & O’Malley, P. M. (1990). Explaining the recent decline in cocaine use among young adults: Further evidence that perceived risks and disapproval lead to reduced drug use. Journal of Health and Social Behavior , 31, 173–184.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bachman, J. G., Johnston, L. D., & O’Malley, P. M. (1998). Explaining recent increases in students’ marijuana use: Impacts of perceived risks and disapproval, 1976 through 1996. American Journal of Public Health , 88(6), 887–892.PubMed Bachman, J. G., Johnston, L. D., & O’Malley, P. M. (1998). Explaining recent increases in students’ marijuana use: Impacts of perceived risks and disapproval, 1976 through 1996. American Journal of Public Health , 88(6), 887–892.PubMed
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1994). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 61–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bandura, A. (1994). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. In J. Bryant & D. Zillman (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 61–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Bandura, A. (1986). The social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1986). The social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
go back to reference Bentler, P. M. (2004). EQS 6 Structural equation manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc. Bentler, P. M. (2004). EQS 6 Structural equation manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc.
go back to reference Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry , 160(6), 1041–1052.PubMedCrossRef Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry , 160(6), 1041–1052.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dackis, C., & O’Brien, C. (2005). Neurobiology of addiction: Treatment and public policy ramifications. Nature Neuroscience , 8(11), 1431–1436.PubMedCrossRef Dackis, C., & O’Brien, C. (2005). Neurobiology of addiction: Treatment and public policy ramifications. Nature Neuroscience , 8(11), 1431–1436.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Donohew, R. L., Hoyle, R., Clayton, R., Skinner, W., Colon, S., & Rice, R. (1999). Sensation seeking and drug use by adolescents and their friends: Models for marijuana and alcohol. Journal of Studies on Alcohol , 60(5), 622–631.PubMed Donohew, R. L., Hoyle, R., Clayton, R., Skinner, W., Colon, S., & Rice, R. (1999). Sensation seeking and drug use by adolescents and their friends: Models for marijuana and alcohol. Journal of Studies on Alcohol , 60(5), 622–631.PubMed
go back to reference Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A. S., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making , 13, 1–17.CrossRef Finucane, M. L., Alhakami, A. S., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. M. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making , 13, 1–17.CrossRef
go back to reference Fishbein M. (1997). Predicting, understanding, and changing socially relevant behaviors: Lessons learnt. In C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psychology (pp. 177–191). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell. Fishbein M. (1997). Predicting, understanding, and changing socially relevant behaviors: Lessons learnt. In C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psychology (pp. 177–191). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
go back to reference Fishbein, M. (2003). Toward an understanding of the role of perceived risk in HIV prevention research. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 49–55). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Fishbein, M. (2003). Toward an understanding of the role of perceived risk in HIV prevention research. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 49–55). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
go back to reference Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of Neurocytology , 31, 373–385.PubMedCrossRef Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of Neurocytology , 31, 373–385.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Giedd, J. N. (2004). Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Science , 1021, 77–85.CrossRef Giedd, J. N. (2004). Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Science , 1021, 77–85.CrossRef
go back to reference Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience , 2(10), 861–863.PubMedCrossRef Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience , 2(10), 861–863.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gullone, E., Moore, S., Moss, S., & Boyd, C. (2000). The adolescent risk-taking questionnaire: Development and psychometric evaluation. Journal of Adolescent Research , 15(2), 231–250.CrossRef Gullone, E., Moore, S., Moss, S., & Boyd, C. (2000). The adolescent risk-taking questionnaire: Development and psychometric evaluation. Journal of Adolescent Research , 15(2), 231–250.CrossRef
go back to reference Hornik R., Maklan, D., Cadell, D., Barmada, C. B., Jacobsohn, L. & Prado, A. (2002). Evaluation of the national youth anti-drug media campaign: Fifth semi-annual report of findings. Rockville, MD: Westat. Hornik R., Maklan, D., Cadell, D., Barmada, C. B., Jacobsohn, L. & Prado, A. (2002). Evaluation of the national youth anti-drug media campaign: Fifth semi-annual report of findings. Rockville, MD: Westat.
go back to reference Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences , 32, 401–414.CrossRef Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences , 32, 401–414.CrossRef
go back to reference Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 76–99). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 76–99). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
go back to reference Jamieson, P. E., & Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2006). Do films about mentally disturbed characters promote ineffective coping in vulnerable youth? Journal of Adolescence, 29, 749–760.PubMedCrossRef Jamieson, P. E., & Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2006). Do films about mentally disturbed characters promote ineffective coping in vulnerable youth? Journal of Adolescence, 29, 749–760.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Johnston, L. (2003). Alcohol and illicit drugs: The role of risk perceptions. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 56–74). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Johnston, L. (2003). Alcohol and illicit drugs: The role of risk perceptions. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 56–74). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Johnston, L. O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2004). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2003. Vol. I: Secondary school students (NIH Publication No. 04-5507). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. Johnston, L. O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2004). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2003. Vol. I: Secondary school students (NIH Publication No. 04-5507). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
go back to reference Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press. Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Krawczyk, D. (2002). Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural basis of human decision making. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 26, 631–664.PubMedCrossRef Krawczyk, D. (2002). Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural basis of human decision making. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 26, 631–664.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin , 127, 267–286.PubMedCrossRef Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin , 127, 267–286.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Maruyama, G. M. (1998). Basics of structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Maruyama, G. M. (1998). Basics of structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
go back to reference Millstein, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2002). Perceptions of risk and vulnerability. Journal of Adolescent Health , 31S, 10–27.CrossRef Millstein, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2002). Perceptions of risk and vulnerability. Journal of Adolescent Health , 31S, 10–27.CrossRef
go back to reference Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review , 100, 674–701.PubMedCrossRef Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review , 100, 674–701.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain-behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology , 14, 499–520.PubMedCrossRef Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain-behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology , 14, 499–520.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Pate, R. R., Heath, G. W., Dowda, M., & Trost, S. G. (1996). Associations between physical activity and other health behaviors in a representative sample of US adolescents. American Journal of Public Health , 86(11), 1577–1581.PubMedCrossRef Pate, R. R., Heath, G. W., Dowda, M., & Trost, S. G. (1996). Associations between physical activity and other health behaviors in a representative sample of US adolescents. American Journal of Public Health , 86(11), 1577–1581.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Quadrel, M. J., Fischhoff, B., & Davis, W. (1993). Adolescent (in)vulnerability. American Psychologist , 48(2), 102–116.PubMedCrossRef Quadrel, M. J., Fischhoff, B., & Davis, W. (1993). Adolescent (in)vulnerability. American Psychologist , 48(2), 102–116.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rather, B. C., & Goldman, M. S. (1994). Drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology , 2, 167–183.CrossRef Rather, B. C., & Goldman, M. S. (1994). Drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology , 2, 167–183.CrossRef
go back to reference Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality , 38, 256–279.CrossRef Roberti, J. W. (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality , 38, 256–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Rodgers, J. L. (2003). EMOSA sexuality models, memes, and the tipping point: Policy and program implications. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 185–192). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Rodgers, J. L. (2003). EMOSA sexuality models, memes, and the tipping point: Policy and program implications. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 185–192). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Rolison, M. R., & Scherman, A. (2002). Factors influencing adolescents’ decisions to engage in risk-taking behavior. Adolescence , 37, 585–596.PubMed Rolison, M. R., & Scherman, A. (2002). Factors influencing adolescents’ decisions to engage in risk-taking behavior. Adolescence , 37, 585–596.PubMed
go back to reference Romer, D. (2003). Prospects for an integrated approach to adolescent risk reduction. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 1–8). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Romer, D. (2003). Prospects for an integrated approach to adolescent risk reduction. In D. Romer (Ed.), Reducing adolescent risk: Toward an integrated approach (pp. 1–8). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Romer, D., & Jamieson, P. (2001). The role of perceived risk in starting and stopping smoking. In P. Slovic (Ed.), Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy (pp. 64–80). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Romer, D., & Jamieson, P. (2001). The role of perceived risk in starting and stopping smoking. In P. Slovic (Ed.), Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy (pp. 64–80). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Romer, D., & Stanton, B. (2003). Feelings about risk and the epidemic diffusion of adolescent sexual behavior. Prevention Science , 4(1), 39–53.PubMedCrossRef Romer, D., & Stanton, B. (2003). Feelings about risk and the epidemic diffusion of adolescent sexual behavior. Prevention Science , 4(1), 39–53.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sallis, J. F., Prochaska, J. J., & Taylor, W. C. (2000). A review of correlates of physical activity of children and adolescents. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise , 32(5), 963–975.CrossRef Sallis, J. F., Prochaska, J. J., & Taylor, W. C. (2000). A review of correlates of physical activity of children and adolescents. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise , 32(5), 963–975.CrossRef
go back to reference Shafer, J., & Brown, S. A. (1991). Marijuana and cocaine effect expectancies and drug use patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology , 59, 558–565.CrossRef Shafer, J., & Brown, S. A. (1991). Marijuana and cocaine effect expectancies and drug use patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology , 59, 558–565.CrossRef
go back to reference Short, L., & Hennessy, M. (2004). Using structural equations to estimate the effects of behavioral interventions. Structural Equation Modeling , 1, 68–81.CrossRef Short, L., & Hennessy, M. (2004). Using structural equations to estimate the effects of behavioral interventions. Structural Equation Modeling , 1, 68–81.CrossRef
go back to reference Slovic, P. (2001). Cigarette smokers: Rational actors or rational fools? In P. Slovic (Ed.), Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy (pp. 97–126). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press. Slovic, P. (2001). Cigarette smokers: Rational actors or rational fools? In P. Slovic (Ed.), Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy (pp. 97–126). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
go back to reference Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Intuitive judgment: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press. Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Intuitive judgment: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Smith, A. M. A., & Rosenthal, D. (1995). Adolescents’ perceptions of their risk environment. Journal of Adolescence , 18, 229–245.CrossRef Smith, A. M. A., & Rosenthal, D. (1995). Adolescents’ perceptions of their risk environment. Journal of Adolescence , 18, 229–245.CrossRef
go back to reference Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Tessner, K. D., & Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. The Journal of Neuroscience , 2(22), 8819–8829. Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Tessner, K. D., & Toga, A. W. (2001). Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. The Journal of Neuroscience , 2(22), 8819–8829.
go back to reference Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 24, 417–463.PubMedCrossRef Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 24, 417–463.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Stephenson, M. T., Hoyle, R. H., Palmgreen, P., & Slater, M. D. (2003). Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence , 72, 279–286.PubMedCrossRef Stephenson, M. T., Hoyle, R. H., Palmgreen, P., & Slater, M. D. (2003). Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence , 72, 279–286.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sutoo, D. E., & Akiyama, K. (1996). The mechanism by which exercise modifies brain function. Physiology and Behavior , 60, 177–181.PubMedCrossRef Sutoo, D. E., & Akiyama, K. (1996). The mechanism by which exercise modifies brain function. Physiology and Behavior , 60, 177–181.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wallace, J. M., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., Schulenberg, J. E., Cooper, S. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2003). Gender and ethnic differences in smoking, drinking and illicit drug use among American 8th, 10th and 12th grade students, 1976–2000. Addiction , 98, 225–234.PubMedCrossRef Wallace, J. M., Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., Schulenberg, J. E., Cooper, S. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2003). Gender and ethnic differences in smoking, drinking and illicit drug use among American 8th, 10th and 12th grade students, 1976–2000. Addiction , 98, 225–234.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yanovitzky, I. (2005). Sensation seeking and adolescent drug use: The mediating role of association with deviant peers and pro-drug discussions. Health Communication , 17(1), 67–89.PubMedCrossRef Yanovitzky, I. (2005). Sensation seeking and adolescent drug use: The mediating role of association with deviant peers and pro-drug discussions. Health Communication , 17(1), 67–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yanovitzky, I. (2006). Sensation seeking and alcohol use by college students: Examining multiple pathways of effects. Journal of Health Communication , 11, 269–280.PubMedCrossRef Yanovitzky, I. (2006). Sensation seeking and alcohol use by college students: Examining multiple pathways of effects. Journal of Health Communication , 11, 269–280.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Yuan, K. H., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Normal theory based statistics in structural equation modeling. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology , 51, 289–309.PubMed Yuan, K. H., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Normal theory based statistics in structural equation modeling. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology , 51, 289–309.PubMed
go back to reference Yuan, K. H., & Bentler, P. M. (2000). Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociological methodology 2000 (pp. 165–200). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association. Yuan, K. H., & Bentler, P. M. (2000). Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociological methodology 2000 (pp. 165–200). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.
go back to reference Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist , 35, 151–175.CrossRef Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist , 35, 151–175.CrossRef
go back to reference Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 10(6), 224–228.CrossRef Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 10(6), 224–228.CrossRef
go back to reference Zuckerman M., Eysenck S., & Eysenck H. J. (1978). Sensation seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural age and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology , 46, 139–149.PubMedCrossRef Zuckerman M., Eysenck S., & Eysenck H. J. (1978). Sensation seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural age and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology , 46, 139–149.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press.
go back to reference Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality , 68, 999–1029.PubMedCrossRef Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk taking: Common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality , 68, 999–1029.PubMedCrossRef
Metadata
Title
A Biosocial-Affect Model of Adolescent Sensation Seeking: The Role of Affect Evaluation and Peer-Group Influence in Adolescent Drug Use
Authors
Daniel Romer
Michael Hennessy
Publication date
01-06-2007
Published in
Prevention Science / Issue 2/2007
Print ISSN: 1389-4986
Electronic ISSN: 1573-6695
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-007-0064-7

Other articles of this Issue 2/2007

Prevention Science 2/2007 Go to the issue