Skip to main content
Log in

Does ambiguity aversion influence the framing effect during decision making?

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Decision-makers present a systematic tendency to avoid ambiguous options for which the level of risk is unknown. This ambiguity aversion is one of the most striking decision-making biases. Given that human choices strongly depend on the options’ presentation, the purpose of the present study was to examine whether ambiguity aversion influences the framing effect during decision making. We designed a new financial decision-making task involving the manipulation of both frame and uncertainty levels. Thirty-seven participants had to choose between a sure option and a gamble depicting either clear or ambiguous probabilities. The results revealed a clear preference for the sure option in the ambiguity condition regardless of frame. However, participants presented a framing effect in both the risk and ambiguity conditions. Indeed, the framing effect was bidirectional in the risk condition and unidirectional in the ambiguity condition given that it did not involve preference reversal but only a more extreme choice tendency.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Camerer, C., & Weber, M. (1992). Recent developments in modeling preferences: Uncertainty and ambiguity. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 325–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassotti, M., Habib, M., Poirel, N., Aïte, A., Houdé, O., & Moutier, S. (2012). Positive emotional context eliminates framing effect in decision making. Emotion, 12(5), 926–931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313, 684–687.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Martino, B., Harrison, N. A., Knafo, S., Bird, G., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Explaining enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(42), 10746–10750.

  • De Martino, B., Camerer, C. F., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(8), 3788–3792.

  • De Neys, W. (2006). Automatic-heuristic and executive-analytic processing during reasoning: Chronometric and dual-task considerations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 1070–1100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 28–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity and Savage axioms. Quaterly Journal of Economics, 75, 643–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Thinking twice: Two minds in one brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, J. L. Y., Keller, L. R., & Keltika, P. (2002). Effects of outcome and probabilistic ambiguity on managerial choices. The Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 24(1), 47–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, M., Bhatt, M., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Camerer, C. F. (2005). Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science, 310, 1680–1683.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huettel, S. A., Stowe, C. J., Gordon, E. M., Warner, B. T., & Platt, M. L. (2006). Neural signatures of economic prefer- ences for risk and ambiguity. Neuron, 49, 765–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inukai, K., & Takahashi, T. (2009). Decision under ambiguity: Effects of sign and magnitude. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119, 1170–1178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, B. E., & Sarin, R. K. (1988). Modeling ambiguity in decisions under uncertainty. Journal on Consumer Research, 15, 265–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective of judgment and choice. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2007). Frames and brains: Elicitation and control of response tendencies. Cognitive Science, 2, 45–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1983). Choices, values and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keren, G., & Gerritsen, E. M. L. (1999). On the robustness and possible accounts of ambiguity aversion. Acta Psychologica, 103, 149–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, I., Snell, J., Nelson, A. J., Rustichini, A., & Glimcher, P. W. (2010). Neural representation of subjective value under risk and ambiguity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(2), 1036–1047.

  • Loewenstein, G., Rick, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Neuroeconomics. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 647–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pulford, B. D., & Colman, A. M. (2008). Size doesn’t really matter: Ambiguity Aversion in Ellsberg Urns with Few Balls. Experimental Psychology, 55, 31–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F. (2004). How people make decisions that involve risk: A dual-processes approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 60–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna, V. F. (2012). A new intuitionism: Meaning, memory, and development in fuzzy-trace theory. Judgment and Decision Making, 7(3), 332–359.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubaltelli, E., Rumiati, R., & Slovic, S. (2010). Do ambiguity avoidance and the comparative ignorance hypothesis depend on people’s affective reactions? Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 40(3), 243–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K., Dickhaut, J., McCabe, K., & Pardo, J. V. (2002). Neuronal substrates for choice under ambiguity, risk, gain, and losses. Management Science, 48(6), 711–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, S. J., Mullette-Gillman, O. A., McLaurin, E. E., Kuhn, C. M., LaBar, K. S., Platt, M. L., & Huettel, S. A. (2011). Low- and high-testosterone individuals exhibit decreased aversion to economic risk. Psychological Science, 22(4), 447–453.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tymula, A., Rosenberg Belkamer, L. A., Roy, A. K., Ruderman, L., Manson, K., Glimcher, P. W., & Levy, I. (2012). Adolesxents’s risk taking behavior is driven by tolerance to ambiguity. Proceeding of the Nationnal Academy of Sciences, 109(42), 17135–17140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X. T. (1996). Framing effects: Dynamics and task domains. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68, 145–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng, H., Wang, X. T., & Zhu, L. (2010). Framing effects: Behavioral dynamics and neural basis. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3198–3204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mathieu Cassotti.

Additional information

A. Osmont and M. Cassotti contributed equally to this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Osmont, A., Cassotti, M., Agogué, M. et al. Does ambiguity aversion influence the framing effect during decision making?. Psychon Bull Rev 22, 572–577 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0688-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0688-0

Keywords

Navigation