The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

M ultiple sclerosis is the most common central nervous system disease affecting young and middle-aged adults. Depression and cognitive dysfunction are common neuropsychiatric symptoms in multiple sclerosis (MS). Lifetime risk of major depressive disorder in MS is around 50%, 1 and a 12-month prevalence of major depressive disorder is approximately 25%. 2 Although cognitive impairments occur across domains, deficits in executive functioning are commonly observed in approximately 33% of patients with MS. 3

Depression and executive dysfunction co-occur in MS, 4 , 5 and both may result from lesions in underlying frontosubcortical brain regions. While there is evidence supporting the role of psychosocial factors resulting from the challenges of coping with a chronic and debilitating disease, 6 demyelinating brain lesions also appear to increase risk of depression significantly. 712 Furthermore, depression is more common among patients with MS than many other chronic or neurological conditions, including spinal cord injury and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. 13

Executive dysfunction has been associated not only with depression, but with response to treatment. The majority of the efforts evaluating the role of executive functioning as a moderator of outcomes for depression have been conducted in the domain of pharmacological treatment in late-life depression, where deficits in executive functioning predict poor response and increased relapse rates in clinical trials evaluating conventional antidepressant treatments. 1416 In addition, Potter et al. 17 found that deficits in executive functioning predicted decreased rates of remission after 3 months of treatment among patients participating in a standardized open-treatment algorithm paradigm. In the general population, deficits in executive functions predicted decreased response to fluoxetine treatment in younger, otherwise healthy individuals with major depressive disorder. 18 In sum, executive functioning appears to play a role in predicting outcomes to antidepressant pharmacological treatments.

Although the pathophysiological mechanisms in late-life depression likely differ from potential mechanisms in MS (i.e., demyelinating lesions resulting in neural dysfunction), there are similarities in the resulting frontosubcortical pathology and impairment. Not only would frontosubcortical pathology precipitate both symptoms of depression and executive dysfunction, 19 it may influence antidepressant treatment response. 20 , 21 Although one study published by our group 12 observed that an association between brain lesions and treatment outcome for depression in MS was mediated by global cognitive functioning, no studies have evaluated specific cognitive markers as predictors of treatment response in MS. In addition, no studies have evaluated cognitive markers in the context of nonpharmacological treatment interventions.

The present investigation is a secondary analysis of a comparative outcome trial for the treatment of depression in MS. 22 In this investigation, we evaluated measures of neuropsychological functioning as treatment predictors across three treatments for depression. We hypothesized that performance on measures of executive functioning would predict poorer treatment response to antidepressant therapy, compared to psychosocial treatment groups.

METHOD

Participants

All procedures of the larger clinical trial are described in detail in Mohr et al. 22 Fifty-nine moderately depressed patients with MS participated in a clinical trial that compared three commonly used treatments for depression among medical patients. After a complete description of the study to participants, written informed consent was obtained under the guidelines of the University of California, San Francisco, Committee on Human Research.

Study participants met inclusion and exclusion criteria described below as evaluated by a study psychologist, psychometrist, and board-certified neurologist. Inclusion criteria were: a) a clinically definite diagnosis of MS using the Poser et al. 23 criteria); b) a relapsing-remitting or secondary progressive disease course confirmed by a neurologist; 23 c) a diagnosis of current major depressive disorder based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID); 24 d) a score of 16 or more on the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D); 25 e) a score of 16 or more on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI); 26 and f) a willingness to abstain from psychological or pharmacological treatment other than that provided in the study during the treatment period.

Exclusion criteria included: a) DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders, other than major depressive disorder or generalized anxiety disorder (using SCID); b) severe cognitive impairment falling below the fifth percentile in three of six areas of neuropsychological functioning (i.e., attention, concentration, speed of processing, executive functioning, verbal memory, and visual processing); c) severe suicidal ideation, plan, and/or intent; d) corticosteroid treatment within 30 days; e) initiation of treatment with an interferon medication within the previous 2 months; f) current MS exacerbation (criteria: enrollment occurred at least 3 months after onset of symptoms of exacerbation, cessation of steroid treatment for 4 weeks prior to enrollment, and patient no longer reports symptoms related to exacerbation); g) head injury or CNS disorder other than MS; h) current or planned pregnancy; i) impairment in visual acuity precluding assessment using visual neuropsychological stimuli (as evaluated by a neurologist using the Snellen visual acuity chart); and j) current psychological or pharmacological treatment for depression.

Treatments

Participants were randomized to one of three 16-week treatments for depression. This was a single-site clinical trial. After the initial screening evaluation, eligible patients who met criteria were screened again 1 to 4 weeks later. This waiting procedure served as a method of blocking patients. When accrual over a 4-week period exceeded six patients, these patients were assigned to group therapy. When accrual was less than six patients, patients were randomly assigned to either cognitive behavior therapy or sertraline. Although this procedure was not strictly random, it adequately met the need to initiate treatment within a reasonable time period and facilitate enrollment in both individual and group treatments.

Individual cognitive behavior therapy consisted of 16 weekly, 50-minute meetings between the individual and a Ph.D.-level psychologist. This treatment included standard procedures 27 and specific skills for the management of MS-related symptoms and problems (e.g., disability, fatigue management, mild cognitive impairment).

Supportive-expressive group psychotherapy is a model of group therapy for people with medical diagnoses originally developed and validated as an intervention for women with breast cancer. 28 Groups of five to nine patients and two Ph.D.-level psychologists met for 16 weekly 90-minute sessions, which focused on enhancing emotional expression, particularly related to MS, and the social and personal sequelae of the disease.

Sertraline is a commonly used antidepressant medication for MS patients. 29 Treatment was initiated at 50 mg per day. The dosage was increased by 50 mg every 4 weeks until a dosage of 200 mg was reached, or until full remission was achieved as judged by the clinicians. Medication dosage and side effects were evaluated by clinical psychologists in conjunction with neurologists. Participant visits were approximately 10 to 15 minutes every 4 weeks.

Assessment Measures

Neuropsychological Functioning

Brief neuropsychological assessments were conducted on an individual basis by trained evaluators blind to group assignment at the beginning of treatment.

Evaluators assessed memory functioning with the delayed recall trial of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) 30 and the 7/24 Spatial Learning Test (SLT). 31

They assessed executive functioning using the total number of correct answers on the verbal fluency test, 32 and the Stroop color-word test interference time from the word reading trial subtracted from the interference trial. 33 This Stroop index was created in order to control for speed of reading or articulation, and to provide a purer interference measure.

Depression

Symptoms of depression were assessed in a structured interview using the HAM-D, 25 which we used because it represents clinical assessment as opposed to self-report (i.e., the BDI). Interrater reliabilities were consistently above 0.90.

Data Analytic Strategy

Preliminary analyses examined patient characteristics as well as the presence of group differences across demographic, neuropsychological, and depression measures. Repeated measures analyses of variance (rANOVAs) were utilized to determine time (pre- versus post-treatment) by treatment (cognitive behavior therapy versus supportive-expressive group therapy versus sertraline) effects, using an intent-to-treat approach, by including participant data with at least one follow-up assessment.

For the primary analyses, we used repeated measures analyses of covariance (rANCOVAs) with neuropsychological performance as the covariate to determine a possible time (pre- versus post-treatment) x treatment (cognitive behavior therapy versus supportive-expressive group therapy versus sertraline) x covariate (neuropsychological performance) interaction for the neuropsychological performance covariates. To explicate these interactions, we performed within-group hierarchical regression analyses using post-treatment HAM-D as the dependent variable. Pre-treatment HAM-D was entered as the first predictor, followed by those neuropsychological variables that resulted in significant rANCOVA interaction effects.

RESULTS

Preliminary Analyses

Patient demographics and disease characteristics are presented in Table 1 . Disease severity was measured by the Ambulation Index, 34 a standardized neurologist rating of MS severity, ranging from 0 (no impairment) to 10 (bedridden with no mobility).

TABLE 1. Patient and Disease Characteristics
TABLE 1. Patient and Disease Characteristics
Enlarge table

Participants in the three depression treatment conditions did not significantly differ on their demographics (i.e., age, education, and gender), pre-treatment depression scores, disease severity, and neuropsychological performance scores (all p>0.51). As expected, there were significant correlations among neuropsychological functioning and demographics [i.e., age was significantly related to Stroop performance (r=−0.49, p<0.001) and RAVLT performance (r=−0.34, p<0.01); education (years) was significantly associated with verbal fluency (r=0.41, p<0.01); and gender was significantly associated with performance on the RAVLT (r=0.27, p<0.05 Spearman correlations)]. Demographic factors were not associated with outcome variables in this study (i.e., post-treatment HAM-D, all p>0.14), and therefore were not controlled for in subsequent analyses. 35

Attrition and Treatment Response

All participants, including those who dropped out, were used in the intent-to-treat analyses, with the post-treatment HAM-D score representing the last assessment for dropouts. Cell sizes for each treatment group were 20, 24, and 15 for the cognitive behavior therapy, supportive-expressive group therapy, and sertraline groups, respectively. Of the total number of 59, nine (15%) patients eventually dropped out of treatment, leaving 50 treatment completers. No participants in this sample dropped out of the cognitive behavior therapy treatment, six (10%) patients dropped out of the supportive-expressive condition, and three (5%) dropped out of the sertraline condition. Differences in attrition across treatment groups were not significant (p=0.14). There were no differences between participants who completed treatment and those who dropped out on demographic, HAM-D, disease severity, or neuropsychological performance (all p>0.18). There was no change in MS disease severity across treatment (p=0.78), or across treatments (rANOVA p=0.51). Using rANOVA, a significant main effect showed a reduction over time in the HAM-D scores ( F (1, 56)=31.82, p<0.001), but no significant effect for treatment condition (p=0.51) ( Table 2 ).

TABLE 2. Means and Standard Deviations for Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
TABLE 2. Means and Standard Deviations for Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
Enlarge table

Neuropsychological Functioning Predicting Treatment Response

There were significant time (pre- versus post-treatment) x treatment (cognitive behavior therapy versus supportive-expressive group therapy versus sertraline) x covariate (neuropsychological performance) interaction effects for the cognitive performance covariates of Stroop [ F (2, 53)=4.85, p<0.05), η 2 =0.16], RAVLT [ F (2, 53)=6.21, p<0.01, η 2 =0.19], and verbal fluency [ F (2, 53)=3.37, p<0.05, η 2 =0.11], presented in Table 3 . In contrast, the covariate spatial learning test [ F (2, 53)=0.015, p=0.99] did not differentiate outcomes across treatment groups.

TABLE 3. Summary of Repeated Measures ANCOVAs Evaluating Time by Treatment: Covariate Is Neuropsychological Variable
TABLE 3. Summary of Repeated Measures ANCOVAs Evaluating Time by Treatment: Covariate Is Neuropsychological Variable
Enlarge table

The within-treatment hierarchical regression analyses were performed to explicate the interactions effects for the Stroop, RAVLT and verbal fluency ( Table 4 ). The Stroop predicted 28.7% (p<0.05) of the variance in residualized post-treatment HAM-D scores in patients receiving sertraline, indicating that poorer performance on Stroop predicted poorer treatment response. In contrast, the Stroop predicted less than 1% of the change in HAM-D scores among patients receiving cognitive behavior therapy (p=0.72), and 9.9% of the variance in supportive-expressive group therapy (p=0.10) treatments. Performance on the RAVLT and verbal fluency accounted for 26.1% (p<0.01) and 25.7% (p<0.01), respectively, of the variance in residualized post-treatment HAM-D scores in the supportive-expressive group therapy treatment group. In contrast to the Stroop findings, poorer performances on the RAVLT and verbal fluency predicted improvement in HAM-D scores in the supportive-expressive therapy group. In comparison, the RAVLT accounted for 6.8% (p=0.36) and 3.6% (p=0.43) of the variance in post-treatment depression scores in the sertraline and cognitive behavior therapy groups, respectively. Verbal fluency only accounted for 7.6% (p=0.33) and 7.2% (p=0.26) of the variance in post-treatment depression scores in the sertraline and cognitive behavior therapy groups, respectively.

TABLE 4. Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses across Treatment Conditions: Neuropsychological Variable Predicting Post-treatment HAM-D Scores
TABLE 4. Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses across Treatment Conditions: Neuropsychological Variable Predicting Post-treatment HAM-D Scores
Enlarge table

DISCUSSION

Our primary hypothesis, that performance on measures of executive functioning would predict poorer treatment response to a 16-week course of antidepressant treatment, was partially supported. This effect was specific to executive functions as measured by Stroop tasks relative to other areas of executive (verbal fluency) or other neuropsychological functioning (memory). These results suggest that there may be differential executive function predictors for treatment response, and that this effect may be specific to cognitive processes evaluated by Stroop tasks, which include response inhibition, conflict resolution, and general complex cognitive processing.

These preliminary findings are largely consistent with investigations in geriatric depression and younger patients with major depressive disorder, finding measures of executive functioning as predictors of antidepressant treatment response. 15 , 16 , 36 Although fluency measures using perseverative error rates also predicted reduced remission rates among patients enrolled in a standardized treatment algorithm, 17 we did not find an effect for fluency in this investigation. However, we evaluated total fluency scores in our sample and did not conduct error analyses which may prove more sensitive. A recent study evaluating a nongeriatric cohort with major depressive disorder found that performance on cognitively complex tasks predicted treatment response to antidepressant therapies. 37 Although Stroop tasks were not used in this investigation, the complex nature of the Stroop may partially explain our effect. In sum, the findings from this investigation extend previous findings by suggesting that neuropsychological deficits may be a similar prognostic indicator in other populations, such as MS. This phenomenon may be independent of specific disease state or syndrome and more strongly related to underlying neuropathology common to these disparate disorders.

Our finding that performance on the Stroop task is related to depression and treatment prognosis is hypothesis-generating regarding potential pathophysiological contributions. Dysfunction in fronto-subcortical brain regions is associated with both depressive symptoms and executive dysfunction. 38 Performance on Stroop tasks can indicate dysfunction in frontal regions, including anterior cingulate circuitry, 39 an area currently under study as a potential mediator of antidepressant response.

Imaging studies also provide clues regarding the role of the anterior cingulate in treatment response. In late-life depression, structural imaging studies found that among poor treatment responders, performance on response inhibition tasks was associated with microstructural abnormalities in the anterior cingulate. 36 Anterior cingulate dysfunction may influence treatment effects by disrupting necessary cortical-limbic functions required to facilitate response to pharmacotherapy. 20 , 21 In psychotherapy, response to treatment may depend less on the integrity of the anterior cingulate, but rely on other neural systems, 40 which may explain why performance on response inhibition tasks failed to predict outcomes in our psychotherapy conditions.

Our findings that poorer performance on other executive measures (i.e., fluency) and that verbal memory predicted improved treatment response in the supportive-expressive group therapy condition is both novel and unexpected. It is possible that this finding is unique to our sample; however, the consistency across measures suggests that this effect on psychotherapy outcomes is worthy of further study. As a result of cognitive deficits, these patients may be particularly responsive to emotional support available in group treatments as compared to conceptually difficult treatment strategies (e.g., cognitive interventions). The group treatment relied upon emotional expression and social support as the vehicle for change. 28 We also speculate that the greater improvement among patients with cognitive deficits is rooted in the social consequences of these impairments. As cognitive dysfunction increases among MS patients, social support deteriorates. 31 Therefore, a treatment providing social support can significantly improve well-being for cognitively impaired patients without adequate support. 41 This argument may also apply to the medication treatment group, as they received far less social contact than the group treatment condition. Therefore, the role of social support in mediating treatment response in patients with cognitive impairments deserves further investigation.

There are several limitations to this study. First, the sample sizes are small, and further investigations replicating these preliminary findings are warranted. An additional limitation is the “post-hoc” nature of this investigation. Prospective investigations of cognitive predictors of depression treatments in MS are necessary. In addition, we were unable to fully characterize depressive disorders in our cohort, including data on family history and onset of depressive disorder in relation to onset of MS. Frontosubcortical structural alterations are associated with MS lesions, but also with familial major depression. 42 These structural changes may precipitate and predispose patients to developing depressive disorders, and may influence response to antidepressant medications. 43 , 44 Future studies better characterizing depressive disorders in MS will help to elucidate etiological factors and predictors of treatment response among MS patients with depression.

To our knowledge, this is the first investigation evaluating depression and neuropsychological predictors of depression treatment outcomes in MS. We hypothesize that both depression and the executive dysfuction may be precipitated by an underlying brain dysfunction contributing to poor antidepressant response. The relationship among executive dysfunction and antidepressant effects in MS needs to be studied prospectively, along with further evaluation of specific brain regions associated with executive functioning, depression, and treatment, in order to clarify these hypothesized brain-behavior relationships.

Received October 27, 2004; revised July 16, 2005; accepted August 9, 2005. Drs. Julian and Mohr are affiliated with the Department of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California. Address correspondence to Dr. Julian, 3333 California St., Suite 270, San Francisco, CA 94143-0920; [email protected] (E-mail).

Copyright © 2006 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

References

1. Sadovnick AD, Remick RA, Allen J, et al: Depression and multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1996; 46:628–632Google Scholar

2. Patten SB, Beck CA, Williams JV, et al: Major depression in multiple sclerosis: a population-based perspective. Neurology 2003; 61:1524–1527Google Scholar

3. McIntosh-Michaelis SA, Roberts MH, Wilkinson SM, et al: The prevalence of cognitive impairment in a community survey of multiple sclerosis. Br J Clin Psychol 1991; 30(pt 4):333–348Google Scholar

4. Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Randolph JJ: Depression in multiple sclerosis: relationship to planning ability. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2001; 7:665–674Google Scholar

5. Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Voss WD, et al. Depression in multiple sclerosis: relationship to working memory capacity. Neuropsychology 1999; 13:546–556Google Scholar

6. Pakenham KI: Adjustment to multiple sclerosis: application of a stress and coping model. Health Psychol 1999; 18:383–392Google Scholar

7. Feinstein A, Roy P, Lobaugh N, et al: Structural brain abnormalities in multiple sclerosis patients with major depression. Neurology 2004; 62:586–590Google Scholar

8. Bakshi R, Czarnecki D, Shaikh ZA, et al: Brain MRI lesions and atrophy are related to depression in multiple sclerosis. Neuroreport 2000; 11:1153–1158Google Scholar

9. Pujol J, Bello J, Deus J, et al: Lesions in the left arcuate fasciculus region and depressive symptoms in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 1997; 49:1105–1110Google Scholar

10. Zorzon M, de Masi R, Nasuelli D, et al: Depression and anxiety in multiple sclerosis: a clinical and MRI study in 95 subjects. J Neurol 2001; 248:416–421Google Scholar

11. Zorzon M, Zivadinov R, Nasuelli D, et al. Depressive symptoms and MRI changes in multiple sclerosis. Eur J Neurol 2002; 9:491–496Google Scholar

12. Mohr DC, Epstein L, Luks TL, et al: Brain lesion volume and neuropsychological function predict efficacy of treatment for depression in multiple sclerosis. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003; 71:1017–1024Google Scholar

13. Schiffer RB, Babigian HM: Behavioral disorders in multiple sclerosis, temporal lobe epilepsy, and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: an epidemiologic study. Arch Neurol 1984; 41:1067–1069Google Scholar

14. Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC, et al: Executive dysfunction and long-term outcomes of geriatric depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:285–290Google Scholar

15. Alexopoulos GS, Kiosses DN, Murphy C, et al: Executive dysfunction, heart disease burden, and remission of geriatric depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:2278–2284Google Scholar

16. Kalayam B, Alexopoulos GS: Prefrontal dysfunction and treatment response in geriatric depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:713–718Google Scholar

17. Potter GG, Kittinger JD, Wagner HR, et al: Prefrontal neuropsychological predictors of treatment remission in late-life depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:2266–2271Google Scholar

18. Dunkin JJ, Leuchter AF, Cook IA, et al: Executive dysfunction predicts nonresponse to fluoxetine in major depression. J Affect Disord 2000; 60:13–23Google Scholar

19. Tekin S, Cummings JL: Frontal-subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. J Psychosom Res 2002; 53:647–654Google Scholar

20. Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Mahurin RK, et al: Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport 1997; 8:1057–1061Google Scholar

21. Mayberg HS: Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997; 9:471–481Google Scholar

22. Mohr DC, Boudewyn AC, Huang L, et al: Comparative outcome trial of three treatments for depression in multiple sclerosis. Ann Behav Med 2000; 22(suppl):S123Google Scholar

23. Poser CM, Paty DW, Scheinberg L, et al: New diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: guidelines for research protocols. Ann Neurol 1983; 13:227–231Google Scholar

24. First MD, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, et al: Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, patient ed. (SCID - I/P), Version 2.0. New York, State Psychiatric Institute, 1995Google Scholar

25. Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62Google Scholar

26. Beck AT, Steer RA: Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, Psychological Corporation, 1993Google Scholar

27. Beck, JS: Cognitive Therapy: Basics and Beyond. New York, NY, Guilford, 1995Google Scholar

28. Spiegel D, Kimerling R: Group psychotherapy for women with breast cancer: relationships among social support, emotional expression, and survival, in Emotion, Social Relationships, and Health. Edited by Ryff C, Singer BH. Oxford University Press, London, 2001, pp 97–123Google Scholar

29. Scott TF, Nussbaum P, McConnell H, et al: Measurement of treatment response to sertraline in depressed multiple sclerosis patients using the Carroll scale. Neurol Res 1995; 17:421–422Google Scholar

30. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment, 2nd Ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 1983Google Scholar

31. Rao SM, Leo GJ, Ellington L, et al: Cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis, II, impact on employment and social functioning. Neurology 1991; 41:692–696Google Scholar

32. Spreen, O, Strauss, E: A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, Second Ed. Oxford New York, Oxford University Press, 1998Google Scholar

33. Dodrill CB: A neuropsychological battery for epilepsy. Epilepsia 1978; 19:611–623Google Scholar

34. Hauser SL, Dawson DM, Lehrich JR, et al: Intensive immunosuppression in progressive multiple sclerosis: a randomized, three-arm study of high-dose intravenous cyclophosphamide, plasma exchange, and ACTH. N Engl J Med 1983; 308:173–180Google Scholar

35. Committee for Proprietary Medicinal Products (CPMP): Points to consider on adjustment for baseline covariates. Stat Med 2004; 23:701–709Google Scholar

36. Alexopoulos GS, Kiosses DN, Choi SJ, et al: Frontal white matter microstructure and treatment response of late-life depression: a preliminary study. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1929–1932Google Scholar

37. Kampf-Sherf O, Zlotogorski Z, Gilboa A, et al: Neuropsychological functioning in major depression and responsiveness to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors antidepressants. J Affect Disord 2004; 82:453–459Google Scholar

38. Soares JC, Mann JJ: The anatomy of mood disorders—review of structural neuroimaging studies. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:86–106Google Scholar

39. Mead LA, Mayer AR, Bobholz JA, et al: Neural basis of the Stroop interference task: response competition or selective attention? J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002; 8:735–742Google Scholar

40. Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, et al: Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:34–41Google Scholar

41. Helgeson VS, Cohen S, Schulz R, et al: Group support interventions for women with breast cancer: who benefits from what? Health Psychol 2000; 19:107–114Google Scholar

42. Drevets WC, Price JL, Simpson JR, et al: Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 1997; 386:824–827Google Scholar

43. Vlassenko A, Sheline YI, Fischer K, et al: Cerebral perfusion response to successful treatment of depression with different serotoninergic agents. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004; 16:360–363Google Scholar

44. Holthoff VA, Beuthien-Baumann B, Zundorf G, et al: Changes in brain metabolism associated with remission in unipolar major depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004; 110:184–194Google Scholar