The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticleFull Access

Brain Lesions in Patients Visiting a Memory Clinic With Postconcussional Sequelae After Mild to Moderate Brain Injury

Published Online:

Abstract

Postconcussional disorder after a relatively mild head injury is common. Although a partial organic etiology is presumed, little imaging evidence exists for this assumption. In this study, patients with mild to moderate brain injury (median Glasgow Coma Scale score of 14) had more parenchymal brain lesions than control subjects (P=0.02). Additionally, the authors investigated the potential of quantifying brain injury by the magnetization transfer ratio (MTR). The curve amplitude of the MTR histogram was used as a measure of normal white matter. Patients had a lower curve amplitude than control subjects (P=0.008). This study provides evidence of persistent traumatic brain alterations in patients who sustained a relatively mild traumatic brain injury.

Modern society places a premium on effective cognitive functioning in both social and professional settings, and even a slight decline in cognitive capabilities can result in significant functional impairment. Head injury is one of the most common causes of cognitive impairment in the young, and the estimated incidence of traumatic brain injury is 200 to 300 per 1 million people in Scotland and the United States.1,2

The majority of head injury is graded as mild, with an initial Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS)3 score of 13 or higher at first assessment.4

Although there is a relationship between the severity of the injury and the ultimate outcome, even mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) without a loss of consciousness can lead to long-lasting cognitive sequelae.57 Many patients complain of headache, dizziness, and concentration problems after mTBI, but within a few weeks these symptoms subside and most patients return to their normal activities. However, 6 months after mTBI, 15% to 29% of patients still have persistent problems,5 and these can continue for years.

Although an at least partially organic origin of the postconcussional disorder is no longer a matter of debate,4,8 a biological marker is still missing, and little imaging evidence exists for this assumption. Oppenheimer9 found microscopic lesions that were probably of traumatic origin in the brains of 5 individuals who sustained mTBI and died of other causes. In laboratory animals, mild injuries caused subtle axonal damage in the absence of gross focal lesions such as contusion or laceration.10

Previous imaging studies with computed tomography (CT) have focused on moderate to severe brain injury, mainly because of the limited sensitivity of CT, which makes the technique less suitable for studying mild to moderate traumatic brain injury. In the acute phase of white matter injury, CT has a sensitivity of only 20%.11 Several studies have shown MRI to be more sensitive than CT,1114 especially in the detection of nonhemorrhagic contusion and axonal injury, lesions commonly found in mTBI.

The application of new MR techniques can further increase the sensitivity of imaging subtle posttraumatic changes. We therefore performed an explorative study in patients with persistent postconcussional sequelae to assess the value of a new MR technique.

Magnetization transfer imaging (MTI) provides higher sensitivity than conventional T2-weighted images in identifying parenchymal lesions. With most conventional MR techniques, image contrast is based on differences between tissue in T1 and T2 relaxation time and on proton density. Magnetization transfer (MT) assessment provides information about the tissue that is different from the information provided by T1 and T2 relaxation times.

The physical principle of MTI is based on the interaction between mobile protons and protons associated with macromolecules.1517 The MRI signal is derived from the water-associated, mobile protons, and the effect of the macromolecular protons is perceived indirectly by transfer of magnetization to mobile protons. This transfer results in a diminished magnetization of these water protons and therefore in a lower signal intensity on the MR image. The MT effect can be quantified by acquiring two images, one with and one without a specific prepulse that maximizes the MT effect, and calculating a suitable ratio (MTR).

Myelin, which is the main constituent of white matter, is considered to be an important macromolecular structure in MT, and loss of macromolecular structure decreases the MT effect. A decrease in MTR is found in the early stages of wallerian degeneration, where conventional imaging sequences fail to detect abnormality.18 MTI also detects lesions in normal-appearing white matter in multiple sclerosis and metastasis.1921 A recent animal study has shown that MTI is sensitive for posttraumatic lesions: areas in the brain that had an altered MT but a normal appearance on T2-weighted scans were found to have axonal disruption and secondary degenerative changes on histopathologic examination.22

Typically the MTR is analyzed in a two-dimensional region of interest. Although this will show focal lesions, diffuse brain abnormalities commonly seen in posttraumatic patients are not easily assessed in this way. Histogram analysis allows for the assessment of a volume of brain tissue and for the quantification of both high-contrast lesions and diffuse abnormalities in normal-appearing white matter.23,24 This method of analysis fully exploits the potential of MTI to detect microscopic lesions.

The present study was conducted 1) to study the relationship between structural brain abnormalities and symptoms after mild to moderate traumatic brain injury, and 2) to assess the potential of MTI for quantifying brain injury. We selected a population with a high a prior risk of abnormalities in order to asses the applicability of this new technique. The subjects were patients with a postconcussional disorder (DSM-IV25) who presented at a university-based memory clinic and matched control subjects.

METHODS

Subjects

Patients with a postconcussional disorder who attended the Memory Clinic of the University Hospital Maastricht were enrolled in the study. The criteria of the DSM-IV classification were used for the diagnosis of postconcussional disorder.25 These criteria include a history of head trauma that caused significant cerebral concussion; evidence from neuropsychological testing or quantified cognitive assessment of difficulty with attention or memory; and three or more of the following complaints: becoming fatigued easily, sleeping disorder, headache, dizziness, irritability, anxiety, depression, affective lability, personality changes, or apathy. These complaints must have commenced, or substantially worsened, after the trauma, and must have affected social or professional functioning. All patients underwent a standardized diagnostic assessment as described elsewhere,26 the elements of which are shown in Table 1. Psychiatric diagnoses were according to DSM-IV criteria.25 All patients were administered the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Ham-D),27 which was used both as a structured symptom checklist for depressive symptoms and, in case of depressive disorder, as a measure of severity. Major psychiatric disorders were excluded on the basis of a clinical interview, and for depression and other affective disorders, by absence of core symptoms of depression as measured with the Ham-D. All patients had sustained a closed head injury at least 1 year before they took part in this study. The GCS score3 recorded in the patient file was used to assess the severity of the trauma. Patients who sustained significant extracranial injury were not included in this study. Furthermore, patients were excluded when a physical, neurological, or psychiatric disorder—including somatic disorders, stroke, epilepsy, neurodegenerative disorders, alcohol abuse, depressive disorder, or psychosis—was diagnosed, or when they used medications with known psychoactive effects. The only psychiatric symptoms accepted were those of the postconcussional disorder. A healthy control group without history of mild brain injury, matched pairwise for sex and age, was recruited from the general population by advertisement in newspapers. The control subjects underwent a neurological and psychiatric examination, and subjects were excluded when a physical, neurological, or psychiatric disorder was diagnosed or when they used medications with known psychoactive effects. Subjects were not reimbursed for their participation. All subjects gave their written informed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Hospital Maastricht.

Neurocognitive Testing

Both the patient and control groups underwent a standardized neuropsychiatric and neuropsychological examination. The neuropsychological assessment included tests for verbal memory, basic cognitive speed, and speed of complex operations. These tests were used because earlier research indicated that these cognitive aspects are most relevant in postconcussional syndromes.28,29 The Word Learning Task (WLT) is based on the Auditory Verbal Learning Test30 and evaluates the ability to acquire and retain new verbal information. Susceptibility to perceptual interference was measured by the Stroop Color Word Test (SCWT),31 which consists of three subtasks: color word naming (I), color naming (II), and naming of color words printed in a different color (interference task, III). The Memory Scanning Test (MST) is designed to study the speed of memory processes.32 The test is based on the principle that additional time is needed to complete a task when there is a stepwise increase in the amount of information to be kept in memory. It reflects the ease with which information is processed in the working memory.

The test results were reduced to three variables by making composite scores for memory, sensorimotor speed, and cognitive speed.33 This was done to reduce the number of dependent variables while improving the robustness of the underlying cognitive construct.31,33,34

The memory score was composed of the results of three WLT tasks: total number of recalled words (WLT1–WLT5), the maximum number of words recalled (TOTG), and the total number of words of the delayed recall (DRCLL). The score of cognitive speed was composed of the slope of the memory scanning test for test with three letters (MSTL3), and the Stroop interference task (STR3). The sensorimotor speed score was calculated by using the basic speed of the memory scanning test (% task MSTPT), the reading speed of the Stroop test (STR1), and the basic speed of memory scanning test of one letter (MSTL1). For transformation into z-scores, the means and the standard deviations of the normative data of different age groups published by Houx34 were used as reference. Care was taken to match the study population with the reference population, but because both groups were compared with the same reference population, the absolute difference between the patients and the control group is independent of the reference population.

Neuroimaging

MR images were acquired on a Philips ACS system operating at 1.5 tesla. A scout sequence was used to align the subsequent scans. The MR examination protocol consisted of an axial proton density and T2-weighted fast spin-echo (FSE) sequence and an axial T2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) sequence (Table 2). MTI was performed with a spin-echo proton density–weighted sequence, with and without an off-resonance prepulse (Table 2). The patients were also studied with a T2-weighted gradient echo sequence (T2 FFE) to detect hemosiderin depositions.

Lesion Analysis

The hard copies of the MRI studies were screened for abnormalities by a neuroradiologist. The lesions were scored on a semiquantitative scale as proposed by Scheltens et al.35 This formal rating scale uses a score of 0–6, with separate scores for each of the cerebral lobes, the periventricular, the subcortical regions, and the infratentorial brain structures. The maximum possible cumulative score for the cerebrum is 24; the maximum score for one region is 6. Perivascular spaces were disregarded. The rater had no knowledge of the clinical data. The reliability of this rating scale was assessed in a previous study.36

Image Processing

The MT images were transmitted to a separate workstation (PowerPC, Apple Macintosh), and we used our own software to calculate an MTR image according to the formula37

MTR=(Moff−Mon)/Moff

where Moff and Mon represent the signal intensity with the saturation prepulse off and on, respectively. In the resulting image, the pixel grayscale value equals the local MTR. From the coronal data set, the slices anterior to the splenium of the corpus callosum were selected for further analysis. This was to ensure that the data set of each individual included the same anatomical regions.

MTR is typically measured in a region of interest (ROI), and the results depend on the position of the ROI. Another approach is to analyze the entire data set by presenting the MTR data as a histogram.23 This method introduces a bias because the MT data of both gray and white matter are analyzed together and a change in gray/white ratio causes a change in the MTR histogram that is unrelated to a change in the MTR. A new postprocessing technique to circumvent this problem has been developed. White matter is segmented from cerebrospinal fluid, gray matter, and extracerebral tissue, and subsequently the histogram of the normalized white matter MTR distribution is analyzed. To characterize this histogram we fitted a Gaussian function to the data, and thus the MTR histogram is characterized by the amplitude, the mean MTR, and the curve width.24

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed by using nonparametric and parametric tests as appropriate. Significance was tested with two-tailed tests, and a P-value of less than 0.05 was considered significant.

RESULTS

Demographics

Thirteen patients (7 men, 6 women) met the criteria for inclusion in the study. The mean age was 40 years (range 21–62 years, SD=13.17). The mean interval between the trauma and presentation was 4 years (range 1–12 years), and the median initial GCS score was 14 (9–15). The median duration of posttraumatic amnesia was 2 hours (0–336), and the median duration of loss of consciousness was 10 minutes (0–1,200). Patients and control subjects were comparable with regard to level of education on a 7-point scale38 (patients 3.9, control subjects 4.2, P>0.1). Although many patients had complaints of mildly lowered mood, only 1 patient had a score on Ham-D in the range of clinically relevant depression (i.e., score >17; mean Ham-D=12.4, range 6–19). The patient with a Ham-D score of 19 had complaints of affective lability, lowered mood, and anxiety, but had no other clinical signs of major depressive disorder.

The subjects of the control group were matched for sex (7 men and 6 women) and age (mean age 40 years, range 20–62 years, SD=13.76; t=0.01, df=24, P=0.99). None of the subjects in the control group had sustained a head injury.

Neuropsychological Tests

The neurocognitive data used to derive the combined z-scores are presented in Table 3. The combined ratings for memory resulted in a z-score of –1.07 (SD=0.98) for the patients and 0.03 (SD=0.93) for the control subjects. The z-scores for sensorimotor and cognitive speed were –4.45 (SD=3.59) and –2.60 (SD=2.83) for patients and –0.32 (SD=0.71) and –0.16 (SD=0.73) for control subjects. The results of the two groups were significantly different on all three cognitive domains (memory: t=2.82, df=22, P=0.01; cognitive speed: t=2.9, df=21, P=0.009; sensorimotor speed: t=3.18, df=16, P=0.006).

Lesions

The analysis of lesions focused on the frontal, temporal, occipital, and parietal lobes. There were too few lesions in the subcortical gray matter and the infratentorial brain to make any inference. In one patient, only the MTI sequence was performed and thus there was no information available about parenchymal lesions. The T2-weighted FFE sequence showed no hemosiderin depositions. The cumulative scores for the different regions showed the frontal–temporal region to be affected most often (Table 4 and Figure 1). The median scores for parenchymal abnormalities in the patient and control groups were 3 and 1, respectively; this difference was significant (Wilcoxon signed-ranks test, z=–2.25, P=0.02). The lesion load in patients is therefore higher than in the control group. Patients had a higher prevalence of parenchymal lesions than control subjects (77% vs. 38%; χ2=3.38, df=1, P=0.066), but this did not reach statistical significance. To assess the effect of age on the volume of parenchymal lesions, the data for the patients and control subjects were combined in an analysis of variance. There was a significant main effect of patient versus control (df=1, F=14.857, P=0.001), and an independent effect of age (df=2, F=9.086, P=0.002). There was also a significant group by age interaction (df=2, F=4.467, P=0.026).

Magnetization Transfer Data

The mean histograms for control subjects and patients are shown in Figure 2. The curve amplitude of the white matter histogram was 83.28 for the patients and 89.76 for the control subjects (Table 5). This difference was statistically significant (t=2.95, df=24, P=0.007). The width also differs significantly between the two groups, but after histogram normalization it contains the same information as the amplitude. The mean MTR for the patients is lower than for the control subjects, but this difference is not significant.

Interaction With Neurocognitive andClinical Parameters

There was no correlation between neurocognitive parameters and the lesion volume or the histogram parameters (Table 6). There was a significant correlation between the lesion volume and the duration of unconsciousness (Pearson correlation coefficient 0.63, P=0.038), and the correlation with duration of posttraumatic amnesia is near significance (Pearson correlation coefficient 0.61, P=0.06).

DISCUSSION

The main finding of our study is an increased lesion load in patients one year or longer after a mild to moderate traumatic brain injury, as evidenced by the lesion load, as well as a significant decreased curve amplitude of the MTR histogram in patients with postconcussional symptoms as compared with matched control subjects. Even in a small group of only 13 patients, these differences were significant. This finding is of importance because it is the first MRI study to provide evidence for an organic etiology of a postconcussional disorder. Jacobs et al.39 showed a correlation between posttraumatic cognitive complains and SPECT findings, but their patients were studied within one year after the trauma, whereas one year was the shortest interval in the present study.

The application of new imaging techniques has shown beyond doubt that mild head injury may lead to brain injury in a much higher percentage than previously thought. In a group of mTBI patients with a negative CT, Mittl et al.14 found 30% abnormal MRI studies, and another study of unselected mTBI patients showed that 77% of the patients had abnormal findings on MRI or SPECT imaging of the brain.40 Even CT reveals hemorrhagic lesions in up to 10% of mTBI patients.41 We provide data that these lesions may persist and are associated with persistent symptoms.

There has been continuous debate on the organic or psychogenic etiology of a postconcussional disorder, and it has become clear that other factors do influence the outcome after a mTBI.4245 Secondary gain and psychosocial factors are important in the etiology of a postconcussional disorder. Clearly, a balanced integrative neuropsychiatric approach is warranted, taking into account all biological, psychological, and environmental factors and their mutual interactions. Because previous imaging showed no abnormalities, it was very difficult to test the hypothesis of brain lesions as an explanation for a postconcussional disorder. However, the current study has added the factor organic brain damage as an etiologic factor for a postconcussional disorder. A prospective study with a larger, unselected sample including patients with mTBI but no complaints is needed to confirm our findings and to establish causality between a postconcussional disorder and traumatic brain lesions.

Demyelination, edema, and posttraumatic changes decrease the MTR,19,20,22 causing a shift toward the left and flattening of the histogram peak. The decreased curve amplitude of the MTR histogram of our patients can therefore be explained by the existence of macroscopic and microscopic lesions. It appears that the curve amplitude is more sensitive to cerebral changes than the mean MTR, since the decreases in the mean MTR were not significant. Histopathologically these lesions are most likely areas of focal axonal loss, demyelination, and astrocytosis.46 Animal studies have shown that mTBI causes mitochondrial swelling, edema, and subtle axonal damage in the absence of gross focal lesions, contusions, or laceration.10,47 This has also been shown in patients who died of a non–CNS-related injury shortly after a minor head trauma.9 The MTR analysis is also more sensitive to cerebral changes than the analysis of the focal lesions. A post hoc analysis shows that after excluding the two subjects with the longest posttraumatic amnesia, reducing the range to 0–48 hours, the difference between patients and control subjects remains significant for the curve amplitude but becomes nonsignificant for the focal lesions.

Our analysis of focal lesions shows that patients with a postconcussional disorder have a higher lesion load than matched control subjects, and that these lesions are mainly located in the frontal and temporal regions. This last finding is in agreement with previous studies.4851 The prevalence of focal lesions in our patient group is 77% as compared with 38% in the matched control group. This is high considering the time elapsed between the trauma and imaging. Parenchymal lesions decrease in size and number after the trauma, especially during the first 3 months.51 MRI studies performed shortly after mTBI show a prevalence of abnormalities ranging from 30% to 77%, depending on the patient selection and scanning technique.14,51,52 The present patient population comprised individuals with a DSM-IV-classified postconcussional disorder with neurocognitive complaints who presented at a university-based memory clinic. This strong selection bias might explain the relatively high prevalence of frontal-temporal lesions. Even though the control subjects had a prevalence of 38% of parenchymal lesions, the patients had both a higher prevalence and a higher lesion volume. It is an interesting finding that differences between patients and control subjects were larger in the older population, as demonstrated by the significant group versus age interaction in the analysis of variance. This finding illustrates the increased vulnerability of the aging brain.

Although lesions were located most predominantly in the frontal and temporal lobes, the size and precise location differed considerably among patients. This heterogeneity is most probably due to differences in trauma mechanisms. The neurocognitive data also showed a heterogeneous image; different cognitive domains were more or less severely affected. It is likely that there is an association between the site of the lesions and the neurocognitive deficit. Levin et al.51 showed an association between lesion location and performance on memory and planning tasks in a group of head injury patients. The majority of patients in that series, however, had sustained moderate head injury. The heterogeneity of both organic lesions and neurocognitive deficits may explain why an association between these was not demonstrated. Posttraumatic amnesia and loss of consciousness are global measures of traumatic brain injury and are known to correlate with the extent of brain injury.53 Therefore, our finding of a correlation between these two measures and the lesion volume is in support of a traumatic origin of the brain lesions.

Our results cannot be extrapolated to all patients with postconcussional symptoms without due consideration. Although we carefully matched for age and education, and excluded patients with concomitant injuries and neurologic or psychiatric disorders, we cannot exclude the effect of other confounding factors, such as litigation.

Interestingly, recent evidence from experiments with healthy individuals who had sustained mTBI a long time ago shows that these subjects suffer from impaired cognitive functioning compared with that of healthy subjects who had not sustained traumatic brain injury.28,54 This finding suggests that mTBI may have long-term subclinical effects that become apparent upon neurocognitive testing. It is tempting to hypothesize that these subtle dysfunctions are due to changes in brain structure similar to those found in our patient group.

CONCLUSIONS

This study provides evidence of posttraumatic brain alterations in patients who sustained a relatively mild traumatic brain injury one or more years before presentation with cognitive complaints at a memory clinic. Our results support the hypothesis of an at least partially organic etiology for postconcussional symptoms in these patients; however, more study is needed to establish causality between a postconcussional disorder and traumatic brain lesions. We are currently carrying out a prospective study to investigate this matter further.

Histogram analysis of MTR data seems a useful method for the detection of diffuse brain injury. The method has the potential to detect macroscopic as well as microscopic lesions and, like every automated analysis, is less prone to observer variation.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. T2-weighted FLAIR images of a 41-year-old male who suffered a mild traumatic brain injury 4 years previously and who presented with neurocognitive complaintsBilateral focal areas with increased signal are seen in the frontal white matter (left and right images) as well as in the left parietal white matter (right image). The T2-weighted gradient-echo images (not shown) did not reveal focal signal loss; there is no hemosiderin deposition.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. White matter magnetization transfer ratio histograms of the patient group and control groupNote the decreased amplitude and the shift to the left of the histogram of the patients.

TABLE 1. Diagnostic procedure in the Maastricht memory clinic
TABLE 2. Scan parameters
TABLE 3. Neurocognitive test results of patients (Pt) and control subjects (Ctr)
TABLE 4. Cumulative score of parenchymal lesions and (in parentheses) the number of affected subjects
TABLE 5. White matter MTR histogram parameters of the patient and control groups
TABLE 6. Pearson correlation coefficients of the neurocognitive parameters with the histogram parameters and lesion volume

Received March 2, 2000; revised January 3, 2001; accepted January 10, 2001. From the Department of Radiology (p.a.m.h., j.t.w.) and Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology (f.r.j.v., j.j., n.r.), University Hospital Maastricht, The Netherlands. Address correspondence to Dr. Hofman, Department of Radiology, University Hospital Maastricht, P.O. Box 5800 6200 AZ Maastricht. E-mail:
References

1 Kraus JF, Black MA, Hessol N, et al: The incidence of acute brain injury and serious impairment in a defined population. Am J Epidemiol 1984; 119:186-201Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Miller JD: Head injury. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993; 56:440-447Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Teasdale G, Jennett B: Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: a practical scale. Lancet 1974; 2(7872):81-84Google Scholar

4 Brown SJ, Fann JR, Grant I: Postconcussional disorder: time to acknowledge a common source of neurobehavioral morbidity. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:15-22LinkGoogle Scholar

5 Bohnen N, Twijnstra A, Jolles J: Persistence of postconcussional symptoms in uncomplicated, mildly head-injured patients: a prospective cohort study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1993; 6:193-200Google Scholar

6 Middelboe T, Birket-Smith M, Andersen HS, et al: Personality traits in patients with postconcussional sequelae. J Personal Disord 1992; 6:246-255CrossrefGoogle Scholar

7 Hugenholtz H, Stuss DT, Stethem LL, et al: How long does it take to recover from a mild concussion? Neurosurgery 1988; 22:853-858Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Lishman WA: Physiogenesis and psychogenesis in the “post-concussional syndrome.” Br J Psychiatry 1988; 153:460-469Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Oppenheimer DR: Microscopic lesions in the brain following head injury. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1968; 31:299-306Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Jane JA, Steward O, Gennarelli TA: Axonal degeneration induced by experimental noninvasive minor head injury. J Neurosurg 1985; 62:96-100Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Orrison WW, Gentry LR, Stimac GK, et al: Blinded comparison of cranial CT and MR in closed head injury evaluation. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1994; 15:351-356MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Hesselink JR, Dowd CF, Healy ME, et al: MR imaging of brain contusions: a comparative study with CT. AJR Am J Roentgenol 1988; 150:1133-1142Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Yokota H, Korokawa A, Otsuka T, et al: Significance of magnetic resonance imaging in acute head injury. J Trauma 1991; 31:351-357Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Mittl RL, Grossman RI, Hiehle JF, et al: Prevalence of MR evidence of diffuse axonal injury in patients with mild head injury and normal CT findings. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1993; 15:1583-1589Google Scholar

15 Wolff SD, Balaban RS: Magnetization transfer contrast (MTC) and tissue water proton relaxation in vivo. Magn Reson Med 1989; 10:135-144Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Hajnal JV, Baudouin CJ, Oatridge A, et al: Design and implementation of magnetization transfer pulse sequences for clinical use. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1992; 16:7-18Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Forsen S, Hofmann RA: Study of moderately rapid chemical exchange reaction by means of nuclear magnetic double resonance. J Chem Phys 1963; 39:2892-2901CrossrefGoogle Scholar

18 Lexa FJ, Grossman RI, Rosenquist AC: MR of wallerian degeneration in the feline visual system: characterization by magnetization transfer rate with histopathologic correlation. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 1994; 15:201-212MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Boorstein JM, Wong KT, Grossman RI, et al: Metastatic lesions of the brain: imaging with magnetization transfer. Radiology 1994; 191:799-803Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Dousset V, Grossman RI, Ramer KN, et al: Experimental allergic encephalomyelitis and multiple sclerosis: lesion characterization with magnetization transfer imaging. Radiology 1992; 182:483-491Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Loevner LA, Grossman RI, Cohen JA, et al: Microscopic disease in normal-appearing white matter on conventional MR images in patients with multiple sclerosis: assessment with magnetization-transfer measurements. Radiology 1995; 196:511-515Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Kimura H, Meaney DF, McGowan JC, et al: Magnetization transfer imaging of diffuse axonal injury following experimental brain injury in the pig: characterization by magnetization transfer ratio with histopathologic correlation. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1996; 20:540-546Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 van Buchem MA, McGowan JC, Kolson DL, et al: Quantitative volumetric magnetization transfer analysis in multiple sclerosis: estimation of macroscopic and microscopic disease burden. Magn Reson Med 1996; 36:632-636Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Hofman PAM, Kemerink G, Jolles J, et al: Quantitative analysis of magnetization transfer images of the brain: the effect of closed head injury, age and sex on white matter. Magn Reson Med 1999; 42:803-806Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1994Google Scholar

26 Verhey FR, Jolles J, Ponds RW, et al: Diagnosing dementia: a comparison between a monodisciplinary and a multidisciplinary approach. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1993; 5:78-85LinkGoogle Scholar

27 Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56-62Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Klein M, Houx P, Jolles J: Long-term persisting cognitive sequelae of traumatic brain injury and the effect of age. J Nerv Ment Dis 1996; 184:459-467Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Bohnen N, Jolles J, Twijnstra A: Neuropsychological deficits in patients with persistent symptoms six months after mild head injury. Neurosurgery 1992; 30:692-696Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Brand N, Jolles J: Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditory and visually. J Gen Psychol 1985; 112:201-210Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 1995Google Scholar

32 Brand N, Jolles J: Information processing in depression and anxiety. Psychol Med 1987; 17:145-153Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 van Boxtel MPJ, Langerak K, Houx PJ, et al: Self-reported physical activity, subjective health, and cognitive performance in older adults. Exp Aging Res 1996; 22:363-379Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Houx PJ: Cognitive aging and health-related factors. Thesis, Maastricht University, 1991Google Scholar

35 Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Leys D, et al: A semiquantitative rating scale for the assessment of signal hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurol Sci 1993; 114:7-12Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Hofman PAM, Wilmink JT, Wielheesen DHM, et al: Fast spin echo and fast FLAIR, a comparative study evaluating parenchymal abnormalities in mood disorders (abstract). Neuroradiology 1997; 39(suppl 1):52Google Scholar

37 Wolff SD, Balaban RS: Magnetisation transfer imaging: practical aspects and clinical applications. Radiology 1994; 192:593-599Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Verhage F: Intelligentie en leeftijd [Intelligence and age]. Groningen, The Netherlands, University of Groningen, 1964Google Scholar

39 Jacobs A, Put E, Ingels M, et al: One-year follow-up of technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT in mild head injury. J Nucl Med 1996; 37:1605-1609MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Hofman P, Stapert S, Kroonenburgh M, et al: MR, SPECT and neurocognitive performance after mild traumatic brain injury. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2001; 22:441-449MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Hofman PAM, Nelemans P, Kemerink GJ, et al: Value of radiological diagnosis of skull fracture in the management of mild head injury: a meta-analysis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000; 68:416-422Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Alexander MR: Mild traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical management. Neurology 1995; 45:1253-1260Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Binder LM: A review of mild head trauma, part II: clinical implications. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1997; 19:432-457Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Binder LM, Rohling ML, Larrabee GJ: A review of mild head trauma, part I: meta-analytic review of neuropsychological studies. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1997; 19:421-431Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Binder LM, Rohling ML: Money matters: a meta-analytic review of the effects of financial incentives on recovery after closed-head injury. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:7-10Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Gean AD: Imaging of Head Trauma. New York, Raven, 1994Google Scholar

47 Bakay L, Lee JC, Lee GC: Experimental cerebral concussion: an electron microscopic study. J Neurosurg 1977; 47:525-531Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Ommaya AK, Gennarelli TA: Cerebral concussion and traumatic unconsciousness: correlation of experimental and clinical observations on blunt head injuries. Brain 1974; 97:633-654Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Mattsom AJ, Levin HS: Frontal lobe dysfunction following closed head injury. J Nerv Ment Dis 1990; 178:282-291Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Levin H, Kraus MF: The frontal lobes and traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:443-454LinkGoogle Scholar

51 Levin HS, Williams DH, Eisenberg HM, et al: Serial MRI and neurobehavioral findings after mild to moderate head injury. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992; 55:255-262Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Kruger J, Vogt J, Stappenbeck C, et al: EEG, CCT und MRT bei Patienten nach leichtem und mittelschwerem Schadel-Hirn-Trauma [EEG, CT and MRI in patients after a mild to moderate traumatic brain injury]. Nervenarzt 1991; 62:226-231MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 Levin HS, Benton AL, Grossman RG: Neurobehavioral Consequences of Closed Head Injury. New York, Oxford University Press, 1982Google Scholar

54 Klein M: Cognitive aging, attention and mild traumatic brain injury. Thesis, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Maastricht University, 1997Google Scholar