INTRODUCTION

Social-cognition—or the cognitive processes that underlie our social interactions—is commonly impaired in individuals with schizophrenia (Penn et al, 1997). Among the processes that are often affected are emotion processing, social perception, theory of mind/mental state attribution, and attributional style/bias (Pinkham et al, 2013). These impairments are important because they are associated with poorfunctional outcome (Horan et al, 2012). As a result, recent attention has focused on treatment strategies for improving social-cognition.

A recent meta-analysis of 19 studies that examined social cognitive training programs in schizophrenia and related disorders found moderate to large effects of training on facial affect recognition (a function of emotion processing), small to moderate effects on theory of mind, and insignificant effects on social perception and attributional style (Kurtz and Richardson, 2012). While training for improved-facial affect recognition has been repeatedly found effective (Statucka and Walder, 2013), the results from training other social cognitive functions have been less consistent. For example,we found that a program of Social-Cognition Skills Training (SCST) was effective for improving facial-affect perception and emotion management but less effective for improving performance on more complex domains such as the theory of mind and attributional bias (Horan et al, 2011b). Considering that the effects of training interventions are variable—both across subjects and across social cognitive domains—we wondered whether the efficacy could be improved with a pharmacological augmentation strategy.

A number of factors led us to select oxytocin (OT) as a candidate compound for facilitating social cognitive training.OT is a peptide that acts as a mediator of pro-social behavior (Insel and Fernald, 2004). There is evidence that OT increases the salience of social information (Averbeck, 2010; Prehn et al, 2013); increases the eye gaze toward eyes on a face (Guastella et al, 2008); selectively improves empathic accuracy in individuals who are less socially proficient (Bartz et al, 2010); and improves clinical symptoms in schizophrenia (Feifel et al, 2010; Lee et al, 2013; Modabbernia et al, 2013). OT has also been reported to improve certain social cognitive processes in schizophrenia (Averbeck et al, 2011; Fischer-Shofty et al, 2013; Goldman et al, 2011; Pedersen et al, 2011). Accordingly, we recently found that single doses of OT led to improvement in higher order inferential processes (Davis et al, 2013), the social-cognition dimension that generally responds less consistently to training than lower-level cue detection (Horan et al, 2009).

Our interest in OT in this current study was not on its direct effects on social cognition, which we did not assess. Rather, we were interested in whether its pro-social effects and its effects on the salience of social information would enhance the effectiveness of a psychosocial intervention, SCST. Because our goal was to evaluate the effects of OT on training, it was important to distinguish any direct effects of OT on social-cognition measures vs effects on learning. Hence, we administered OT only before each SCST session. Our evaluations of social cognition, neurocognition, and clinical psychopathology were administered at least a week after patients received their final dose of OT. As a result, improvements in social cognition and other measures could only be related to learning (augmented by OT) and not to direct drug effects on the outcome measures. In this proof-of-concept study, we used a shortened version of SCST (12 sessions over 6 weeks) that addressed three social cognitive processes (empathy, social perception, and facial affect recognition). We assessed outcome measures for each of these three processes, in addition to measures of other social cognitive process not specifically trained, at baseline, 1 week after completion of training, and at a 4-week follow up.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Subjects included 27 male outpatients who met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia, as confirmed by the Structured-Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. All of the subjects were clinically stable on an antipsychotic medication, with no dose change >10% within 3 months of study entry. Exclusion criteria included inability to perform informed consent, history of hyponatremia, or history of epilepsy or traumatic-brain injury. Subjects were recruited from VA, UCLA, and community outpatient mental health clinics. After complete description of the study to the subjects, written-informed consent was obtained. All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional Review Boards at both UCLA and VA Greater Los Angeles.

Study Design

One week before receiving study drug and beginning training, subjects received a performance battery of social-cognition tasks (described below), the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) (Nuechterlein et al, 2008), and clinical symptom assessments with the Brief Psychiatric-Rating Scale (BPRS) (Overall and Gorham, 1962) and the Clinical-Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS) (Kring et al, 2013). One week later, subjects began SCST training in groups that included individuals receiving OT or placebo. Subjects were randomized to intranasal OT (40 IU) or a matched placebo within each training group. Double-blind drug was administered 30 min before the SCST session and not on any other days. The social-cognition measures and the MCCB were re-administered at 1 week and 1 month after the final SCST session. The BPRS and the CAINS were re-administered at 1 week posttreatment. Adverse events were monitored by a side effect checklist and the Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C-SSRS) (Posner et al, 2011) which were administered at baseline and at 1 week and 1 month posttreatment.

Pharmacological Treatment

OT nasal spray (50 IU/ml) and an otherwise-identical placebo nasal spray were compounded by Inland Compounding Pharmacy (Loma Linda, CA, USA). Nasal sprays were prepared in 30-ml multi-use bottles, calibrated to dispense 0.1 ml per puff. Subjects were instructed to spray four puffs into each nostril, for a total dose of 40 IU OT (or equivalent volume of placebo spray). This dose was selected based on previous studies (Davis et al, 2013; Feifel et al, 2010).

Social Cognitive Skills Training

All of the study subjects received SCST that was similar to the program described in our previous work (Horan et al, 2011a). The program was modified to include only 12 sessions, administered twice a week for 6 weeks. The training was administered in small group format (6–8 participants per group), and because randomization was within training groups, all groups included individuals assigned to both placebo and OT. Training sessions were conducted by two group leaders who were trained and supervised by the developer of the intervention. Four of the sessions focused on interpreting facial expressions and situation (facial affect recognition); four focused on interpreting nonverbal gestures and vocal cues (social perception); and the final four focused on improving empathic accuracy. Each session consisted of 10 min of review, 30 min of training with new material, and 20 min of practice exercises. The training utilized skill-building techniques that are commonly used in psychiatric rehabilitation. These include breaking down complex social cognitive processes into their components and automating these skills through repetition and practice.

Social Cognition Measures

We included three social cognition measures that covered abilities that were a focus of training in the SCST. In addition, we included two social cognitive measures that were used to assess generalization to social cognitive domains not specifically covered in the training.

Measures assessing trained domains. These social-cognition measures included: facial affect recognition: participants were asked to identify facial expressions of basic emotions (happy, sad, angry, afraid, surprised, disgusted, or neutral) in still digital images from the standardized stimulus set developed by Ekman (Ekman and Friesen, 1976). Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (PONS): the PONS (Rosenthal et al, 1979) is used to assess social perception. Scenes of this videotape-based measure last 2 s and contain facial expressions, voice intonations, and/or bodily gestures of a Caucasian female. The empathic accuracy task: in this task (Lee et al, 2011), participants watch 12 (six positive and six negative) video clips, each lasting for 2.0–2.5 min. Each clip shows an individual (referred to as a ‘target’) while he/she discusses a positive or negative autobiographical event. For each clip, participants use a 9-point scale to rate how positive or negative they believe the target is feeling. The primary dependent measure is the correlation between participant ratings of the targets’ emotions and the targets’ ratings of their own emotions, calculated in 2-s time epochs throughout the clip. The mean correlation across clips provides an ‘empathic accuracy’ score for each participant.

Additional social cognitive measures. Managing emotions component of Mayer–Salovey–Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT). The MSCEIT (Mayer et al, 2003) assesses four components (branches) of emotional processing. This study focused on the Managing Emotions component, which has two subscales that examine the regulation of emotions in oneself and in one's relationships with others. The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): The TASIT (McDonald et al, 2003) is a videotape-based test of theory of mind in various social contexts. We used only part three, which assesses the ability to correctly identify sarcasm or lies.

Statistical Analysis

T-tests and Fisher’s exact tests were used for baseline comparisons of age, sex, education level, marital status, and race between treatment groups. Generalized linear-mixed models (GLMM) with main effects of treatment (placebo and OT) and time (baseline, end of treatment, and 1-month follow up), attendance (number of completed training sessions), treatment by time interactions, and subject level random intercepts were used to model the longitudinal trajectories of the outcomes, employing an identity link (using SAS PROC MIXED). GLMM account for correlations between repeated measures within subjects and automatically handle missing data, producing unbiased estimates as long as observations are missing at random. Hence, all available observations from each subject were utilized in modeling via the GLMM. In addition to comparing scores on individual tests, we computed composite scores using the mean of Z-scores of individual measures (which were calculated using entire cohort means and SDs at baseline). These scores were MCCB nonsocial composite (consisting of all individual MCCB components except for MSCEIT-Managing Emotions) and social-cognition composite (consisting of PONS, TASIT, facial affect recognition, empathic accuracy, and MSCEIT-Managing Emotions). The prespecified primary outcome measure of this study was the social-cognition composite score. Estimates of effect size (Cohen’s d) were computed using the difference between the OT and placebo mean changes from baseline to follow up assessments and the pooled (OT+placebo) SD of the change from baseline to follow up assessment.

RESULTS

See Figure 1 for a CONSORT diagram summarizing study screening, recruitment, randomization, and attrition. Thirteen patients were randomly assigned to the OT condition and 14 to placebo. There were no significant demographic differences between the patients assigned to active drug or placebo (Table 1). The number of subjects taking ant cholinergic medications (eg, benztropine, trihexyphenidyl, or diphenhydramine) was similar (3/14 in the placebo group and 2/13 in the OT group). There was no significant difference in group session attendance between groups (mean 8.6 sessions±4.7 SD in the placebo group and mean 9.6 sessions±3.4 SD in the OT group).

Figure 1
figure 1

CONSORT diagram of participants in pharmacological approach to improve the outcome of social-cognition training.

PowerPoint slide

Table 1 Demographic Characteristics

Social Cognition

(see Table 2): On our prespecified primary outcome measure, a social-cognition composite score, we found significant main effects of time, indicating improvement across the combined-treatment groups (p=0.03 at 1 week posttreatment and p<0.01 at 1 month). We also found significant main effects of time on individual measures of facial affect recognition (p<0.0001 at 1 week and 1 month posttreatment); the MSCEIT Managing Emotions total score (p=0.01 at 1 week posttreatment and p=0.03 at 1 month); and the PONS total score (p=0.04 at 1 month posttreatment). On our measure of empathic accuracy we found that subjects assigned to OT demonstrated significantly greater improvements than placebo on the total posttreatment (p=0.03, d=0.92) and at 1 month (p=0.03, d=0.98) (Figure 2). There were no effects of OT on any other individual social-cognitive measures or the social-cognition composite score.

Table 2 Social and Nonsocial Cognitive Assessments
Figure 2
figure 2

Social-cognitive performance on measures trained by SCST. (a) Empathic-accuracy correlation coefficients, (b) Ekman facial-affect recognition fraction correct, and (c) Profile of nonverbal sensitivity total scores for each treatment group at baseline, 1 week posttreatment, and 1 month follow up. *p=0.03 treatment Ă— time interaction. †p<0.0001 main effect of time. ‡p=0.04 main effect of time. All SDs can be found in Table 2.

PowerPoint slide

Neurocognition

(see Table 2): We did not find effects of OT on basic neurocognition as measured by the MCCB nonsocial composite score. There was a main effect of time (across the combined-treatment groups) on the MCCB nonsocial composite score between baseline and 1 month follow up (p<0.01), suggesting overall improvement that may have been related to SCST and/or practice effects.

Clinical Symptoms

Clinical symptoms as measured by the BPRS and the CAINS (for negative symptoms) did not change significantly from baseline to posttreatment or 1 month follow up for either the OT or the placebo-treated patients (Table 3).

Table 3 Clinical Symptom Assessments

Adverse Events

There was one serious adverse event that was determined to be unrelated to study participation. In this case, the participant (who had been randomized to OT) had a recurrence of methamphetamine use that resulted in a brief psychiatric hospitalization for psychosis. He recovered fully and returned to the study groups. The only adverse events among study participants were occasional complaints about nasal irritation from the spray. These were equally distributed between OT and placebo patients. There were no changes in suicidal ideation or behaviors in either group, as assessed with the C-SSRS.

DISCUSSION

Compared with placebo, intranasal OT administered 30 min before SCST led to a significantly greater improvement in empathic accuracy. We are not aware of other psychosocial or pharmacological interventions that have been found to improve the acquisition of empathy skills in individuals with schizophrenia. However, we did not find effects of OT on our other measures of neurocognition and social cognition. Empathic accuracy is a high-level social cognitive skill that involves activation of the mirror neuron system as well as brain regions involved in mental state attribution (Zaki et al, 2009). The improvement in empathic accuracy was seen at the end of the training and after 1 month following OT administration, indicating that the beneficial effects were maintained beyond the acute effects of drug.

We found effects of time (all indicating improvement across the combined-treatment groups) on Ekman facial-affect recognition, MSCEIT total, and the social-cognition composite score between baseline and both posttreatment assessments. The improvement in the social-cognition measures is consistent with prior reports (Horan et al, 2009, 2011b; Roberts and Penn, 2009) finding similar effects. We also found time effects on the nonsocial MCCB composite posttreatment, which is consistent with prior study (Horan et al, 2011b). Though we did not have a control psychosocial intervention in this study, the time effects are most likely owing the SCST intervention, given their consistency with earlier controlled studies. The lack of an effect of social-cognition training on higher level inferential measures of social cognition such as theory of mind is also consistent with studies from the same groups.

There are a number of plausible mechanisms that can explain OT’s effects on the learning of empathic accuracy. If patients treated with OT were less distracted by psychotic thought processes, they could be more attentive participants in training. This would be supported by previous findings that regular OT administration can reduce psychotic symptoms (Feifel et al, 2010). However, we did not find effects of OT on the learning of lower level social-cognition skills such as social perception or emotional processing, which may have occurred if this finding was related to effects on psychosis. Moreover, our previous study did not find acute effects of OT on clinical symptoms of schizophrenia (Davis et al, 2013), so improved attention via decreased psychotic symptoms is unlikely to be the mechanism. It is also possible that OT’s effects on learning could have resulted from nonspecific cognitive-enhancing effects. OT has been reported to improve performance on subtests of the California Verbal Learning Test (Feifel et al, 2012), suggesting that chronic OT may improve verbal memory. Improvement in this basic cognition domain could have led to improved-content retention in the training setting. Again, the lack of effect of OT on lower level social cognitive skills does not support this as a mechanism for OT’s effects.

There is evidence that OT can have effects on important social-cognitive functions which may have facilitated learning. This mechanism is consistent with our recent finding that single doses of OT acutely improve higher level social cognitive processes (Davis et al, 2013) as well as a study that found individuals with schizophrenia showed improvement in judging intimacy and kinship after receiving OT (Fischer-Shofty et al, 2013). It is thus plausible that OT facilitated the learning of social-cognition skills by increasing the salience of social information. Additional support for this mechanism includes evidence that men who were administered a single 24 IU dose of OT recognized facial expressions at a lower intensity level than those administered placebo (Prehn et al, 2013), as well as the finding that OT enhanced pupil dilation in response to emotional faces, suggesting enhanced attention to social stimuli (Leknes et al, 2012). A hypothetical neural mechanism by which OT could increase the salience of social information is by modulating fast-spiking interneurons and thereby increasing the signal-to-noise ratio, as recently reported (Owen et al, 2013). It is not clear why OT only enhanced learning of empathic accuracy in this study and not facial affect recognition or social perception. This could be owing to either insufficient power to detect effects on these processes, or to OT having specific effects on a subset of neural circuits involved in social cognition.

We selected key outcome domains based on the modules that were covered in this 12 session version of SCST. The regular SCST includes 24 sessions and has a module on theory of mind (specifically on recognizing lie and sarcasm), but the abbreviated version used in this study did not. Empathic accuracy relies partially on cognitive empathy, which is similar to theory of mind (Zaki and Ochsner, 2011). However, theory of mind was not specifically trained in the empathy module.

It is important to note that the effects of OT on processing social information can vary depending on individual differences among subjects, including baseline abilities. For example, one study found that the performance-enhancing effects of OT were only seen in women with a lower interest in social interaction (Groppe et al, 2013). This is similar to the finding that OT’s effects on empathic accuracy were only apparent in less socially-proficient individuals (Bartz et al, 2010) as well as that OT’s effects on processing emotional faces were significantly greater in individuals with lower baseline emotional sensitivity (Leknes et al, 2012). It thus follows that OT may facilitate the learning of more complex social skills in individuals with schizophrenia who are limited in their ability to learn owing to difficulties in detecting subtle social signals.

One question that applies to this study, as well as other intranasal OT-treatment studies, is how long a given dose exerts its effects. While pharmacokinetic data regarding intranasal OT is limited, CSF levels of vasopressin (a nonapeptide similar in size and structure to OT) is elevated within 15 min of intranasal administration, with levels still increasing after 75 min (Born et al, 2002). Additionally, it was recently reported that intranasal administration of 24 IU OT (a lower dose than that used in our study) resulted in elevated-plasma OT levels from 15–75 min and elevated-CSF levels at 75 min (with few time points sampled) (Stripes et al, 2013). Though elevated compartmental concentrations do not directly indicate receptor activity, it is reasonable to infer that intranasal OT was exerting a biological effect during the social-cognitive skills training sessions.

Important limitations of this study were the small sample size and our inability to control the possible effects of OT on clinical psychopathology and neurocognition. This was a preliminary study and we did not control for multiple comparisons. Hence, any conclusions from this study are necessarily tentative. Like any results from a preliminary study with multiple comparisons, our findings will await replication from larger samples. Important strengths include the double-blind design and the measurement of social cognition at least 1 week after OT administration. This later advantage suggests that effects of OT were owing to enhanced learning rather than OT’s direct effects on social cognition. We also cannot prove that administering OT twice weekly before training did not cause a sustained increase in endogenous release. OT is known to have feed-forward regulatory properties, and long-term up regulation of endogenous OT systems by exogenous OT has been reported in rodents (Bowen et al, 2011).

Overall, this study demonstrated the feasibility and possible therapeutic benefit of administering OT before a psychosocial intervention targeting social cognition in individuals with schizophrenia. Improving empathic accuracy could help facilitate functional recovery in individuals with schizophrenia, given the significant associations between empathic abilities and social competence, functional capacity, and community functioning (Smith et al, 2013; Smith et al, 2012). These results support further investigation of this novel use of intranasal OT in schizophrenia as well as other psychiatric disorders associated with impaired social-cognitive function.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

Dr Marder has received consulting fees from Abbott, Pfizer, Lundbeck, Bushranger Ingelheim, Bristol Meyers Squibb, Shire, Roche, Genentech, Otsuka, Targacept, and EnVivo. He has received grant supports from Amgen and Sunovion. Dr Green reports having been a consultant to Abbott Laboratories (AbbVie), Biogen, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, and Roche; he is a member of the scientific board for Mnemosyne; and he has received research funds from Amgen. This work was supported by the Hofmann Trust through the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation with an award to Dr Marder. The authors declare no conflict of interest.