Skip to content
Publicly Available Published by De Gruyter October 1, 2016

Pressure and cold pain threshold reference values in a large, young adult, pain-free population

  • Robert Waller EMAIL logo , Anne Julia Smith , Peter Bruce O’Sullivan , Helen Slater , Michele Sterling , Joanne Alexandra McVeigh and Leon Melville Straker

Abstract

Background and aims

Currently there is a lack of large population studies that have investigated pain sensitivity distributions in healthy pain free people. The aims of this study were: (1) to provide sex-specific reference values of pressure and cold pain thresholds in young pain-free adults; (2) to examine the association of potential correlates of pain sensitivity with pain threshold values.

Methods

This study investigated sex specific pressure and cold pain threshold estimates for young pain free adults aged 21–24 years. A cross-sectional design was utilised using participants (n =617) from the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study at the 22-year follow-up. The association of site, sex, height, weight, smoking, health related quality oflife, psychological measures and activity with pain threshold values was examined. Pressure pain threshold (lumbar spine, tibialis anterior, neck and dorsal wrist) and cold pain threshold (dorsal wrist) were assessed using standardised quantitative sensory testing protocols.

Results

Reference values for pressure pain threshold (four body sites) stratified by sex and site, and cold pain threshold (dorsal wrist) stratified by sex are provided. Statistically significant, independent correlates of increased pressure pain sensitivity measures were site (neck, dorsal wrist), sex (female), higher waist-hip ratio and poorer mental health. Statistically significant, independent correlates of increased cold pain sensitivity measures were, sex (female), poorer mental health and smoking.

Conclusions

These data provide the most comprehensive and robust sex specific reference values for pressure pain threshold specific to four body sites and cold pain threshold at the dorsal wrist for young adults aged 21–24 years. Establishing normative values in this young age group is important given that the transition from adolescence to adulthood is a critical temporal period during which trajectories for persistent pain can be established.

Implications

These data will provide an important research resource to enable more accurate profiling and interpretation of pain sensitivity in clinical pain disorders in young adults. The robust and comprehensive data can assist interpretation of future clinical pain studies and provide further insight into the complex associations of pain sensitivity that can be used in future research.

1 Introduction

Quantitative sensory testing (QST) as a measure of pain sensitivity, is being increasingly used to generate somatosensory profiles of patients in clinical pain studies [1, 2] and to measure outcomes in randomised controlled trials [3, 4]. However meaningful interpretation of data from these studies requires appropriate reference values for what is ‘normal’. This ideally should be drawn from large population-based samples of ‘healthy pain-free participants’, adjusted for age, sex, and other potential confounders [5], thereby allowing for generalisability. Currently there is a lack of large population studies that have investigated pain sensitivity distributions in healthy people.

While there are some ‘normative’ datasets against which to reference clinical QST data [6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12], currently there is no comprehensive reference QST data specific to young adults. As the transition from adolescence to adulthood is a critical time during which trajectories for persistent pain can become established [13, 14, 15, 16] there is value in establishing normative data for this young age group. Issues with the utility of current normative datasets for QST include a lack of adherence to recent calls for standardised definitions, and best practice recommendations to adjust for potential confounding variables such as age and sex [17]; datasets that include participants with pain [7, 8] ; having relatively small numbers in each age and sex range [9, 10, 11]; or wide ranging age groups [12]. There is thus a gap in knowledge of normal age-sex specific pain sensitivity distributions.

Further, without large cohort reference values to define ‘normal’ and an understanding of potential correlates, the interpretation of QST measures in pain studies is severely limited, as is their utility in management of people with pain [5, 18]. Potential independent correlates associated with increased pain sensitivity to pressure and cold stimuli include younger age [6], female sex [19], increasing Body Mass Index [6, 20], higher psychological symptoms of depression, anxiety, stress and catastrophizing, [2, 6, 21, 22, 23], decreased health related quality of life [6], lower physical activity and increased sedentary behaviour, [21, 24, 25], and smoking [8, 26]. Only one normative study of pain sensitivity has investigated a broad range of potential correlates (demographic, psychological and health-related factors), but this study was limited by a relatively wide age range with small age-specific participant numbers [6].

Clinically, the assessment of an individual’s pain sensitivity can inform treatment options [27]. In this context, exploring normative ranges for deep tissue pain sensitivity (pressure pain threshold: PPT) is particularly important given deep tissues are implicated in musculoskeletal conditions [28, 29, 30, 31, 32]. With the availability of affordable algometers, there is increasing use of PPT testing in clinical settings to assess and monitor tissue sensitivity levels Cold hypersensitivity (cold pain threshold: CPT) has also demonstrated clinical utility for predicting poor prognosis in whiplash associated disorders [33] and differentiating pain mechanisms in musculoskeletal pain conditions [18, 23, 34, 35].These two clinically relevant nociceptive stimuli can form part of a shorter QST protocol by limiting participant burden and improving time efficiency [36].

The large birth cohort investigated here provided an opportunity to capture more precise sex specific pressure and cold pain threshold estimates for young, pain-free adults. The aims of this study were: (1) to provide sex-specific reference values of pressure and cold pain thresholds in young pain-free adults; (2) to examine the association of site, sex, ethnicity, height, weight, smoking, health related quality of life, psychological factors and physical activity levels with pain threshold values.

2 Methods

2.1 Study Population

Cross-sectional data for this study was obtained from the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study (http://www.rainestudy.org.au). This is an ongoing birth cohort study that commenced with 2900 women who enrolled in the study before the 18th gestation week and 2868 children born, entered the initial birth cohort. Data has been collected at 1,2, 3, 5, 8,10,14,17, 20 and 22 years. The characteristics of the active participants were compared with census data collected in 2011 on all similarly aged young adults in Western Australia. The comparison showed that the sample remains widely representative for a range of variables including education level, employment status, income, marital status, number of offspring, hours worked and occupation. The 22 year follow-up data collection ran between March 2012 and July 2014. Further detail on full measures collected can be found at http://www.rainestudy.org.au/for-researchers/cohort-follow-ups/ [37].

2.2 Recruitment, sampling and data collection

All data used in this study were obtained at the 22 year followup. Data were collected as part of 4 h of testing followed by an overnight sleep study. For this follow up, 1065 individuals participated in pressure pain and cold pain threshold testing. Of the 970 participants who had pressure and cold pain threshold data, and completed questionnaire and physical assessment data on nominated correlates, 617 (280 female and 337 male) were classified as pain free and were included for analysis. Participants were considered pain free if they answered “no” to the question “do you have any current body pain?” from the Orebrö Musculoskeletal Pain Questionnaire (OMPQ).

Questionnaires were filled in before physical assessments and were checked for completion. Anthropometry measures and pressure and cold pain threshold testing were part of the physical assessment protocol conducted by twelve Raine research staff, all of who were thoroughly trained in the data collection procedures and used standardised protocols.

2.3 Quantitative sensory testing

Due to time constraints allowed for collecting data and to minimise the already significant participant burden, the sensitivity measures considered most clinical relevant were collected. A standardised protocol for QST consistent with current best practice recommendations [12, 17], was used to measure PPT and CPT. All QST measurements were taken from the right side of the body, as side to side consistency in pain threshold measurements have been shown in people with [38] and without pain [12]. All testing was done in the early evening minimising the influence of circadian rhythms on pain sensitivity [39] and the order of testing was PPT, followed by CPT, as applying cold first has been found to increase the risk of mechanical hyperalgesia [40]. This testing sequence has been used previously [23]. Both PPT and CPT have demonstrated inter-examiner and intra-subject reliability with reasonable levels of standard error of measurement [41, 42, 43]. Excellent interrater and intrarater reliability for PPT testing by the Raine research staff has been demonstrated, with the caveat that an absence of any confounding of study estimates by rater should be checked due to a systematic rater bias identified [44].

2.4 Pressure pain thresholds

Pressure pain threshold (PPT) was established using a pressure algometer (Somedic AB, Sweden) with a contact area of 1 cm2 applied perpendicularly to the skin with a ramp rate of 50 kPa/s. The ramp rate varies across studies and it is acknowledged slower ramp rates may be appropriate for the study of clinical pain populations [45].PPT was defined as the moment the sensation of pressure became one of pressure and pain. Standardised instructions were read to participants: “The moment the pressure increases to a point where it first feels uncomfortable or painful, press and release the button. This means the very first onset of discomfort or pain and not the most pressure that you can bear”. A cut-off pressure value of 1000 kPa was set for safety purposes [46]. Four trials were performed with a minimum 10 s rest between trials. The mean threshold was calculated for each site from the last three trials.

In order to capture widespread sensitivity data, four standardised sites were tested in the following sequence; the wrist, upper leg, upper trapezius and lumbar spine. These sites have been previously documented [2]. The wrist was tested at the middle of the dorsal aspect of the wrist joint line. The leg was tested at the muscle belly of tibialis anterior, approximately 2.5 cm lateral and 5 cm distal to the tibial tubercle. The upper trapezius was tested at the mid-point between the C7 spinous process and the lateral acromion. The lumbar spine was tested at the erector spinae, 2 cm lateral to the L4/L5 interspinous space.

2.5 Cold pain threshold

To obtain the cold pain threshold, a thermal stimulator (Modular Sensory Analyzer (MSA), Somedic AB, Sweden) with a 12.5 cm2 (25 mm × 50 mm) probe was used at one standardised body site. This site was the middle of the dorsal aspect of the wrist joint line, consistent with the test site for PPT. The baseline temperature was set as 32°C with a cut off temperature of 5°C. The temperature decreased at 1°C/s until the participant first perceived pain and pressed the control switch to terminate the test. For CPT, the following instructions were given to participants “Allow the temperature to drop until the moment it reaches a point where it feels uncomfortably or painfully cold, and then press the button. This means the very first onset of discomfort or pain and not the most cold that you can bear”. Four trials were performed with a 10 second rest period between trials. The mean threshold was calculated from the last three trials.

2.6 Other variables

In order to investigate for possible associations with QST measures, a number of other variables were collected.

2.7 Physical measures

With shoes removed, height (metres) was measured with a stadiometer and body mass (kilograms) was measured with digital scales [47]. BMI (kg/m2) was calculated from these measures. BMI was further categorised into underweight (<18.5 kg/m2), normal (18.5–24.9 kg/m2), over-weight (25.0–29.9 kg/m2) or obese (≥30.0 kg/m2). Waist and hip circumference were measured using a metric tape measure and a standardised protocol, to calculate the waist/hip ratio (WHR).

2.8 Smoking

Subjects were asked, ‘Do you currently smoke cigarettes/cigars?’ and were classified accordingly, as smokers or non-smokers.

2.9 Health-related quality of life

Health-related quality of life was measured using the Short Form-12, version 2 (SF-12) [48], a validated and reliable measure of health related quality of life. Twelve questions produce two summary measures: a Mental Component Summary (MCS); and Physical Component Summary (PCS) [49]. Each SF-12 scale is a norm-based score with a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10, with higher scores indicating less disability [48]. The MCS and PCS of the SF12 were categorised into those with a score ≥50 and <50, to allow comparison with the study by Neziri et al. [6], in which SF36 scores were dichotomised.

2.10 Psychological Data

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale 21 (DASS-21) was used to evaluate the severity of depression, anxiety and stress related symptoms. It is a valid and reliable questionnaire where a higher score indicates greater severity of symptoms [50], with established cut-off scores for mild, moderate, severe and extremely severe levels of each sub-scale [51].

2.11 Activity measures

Sedentary behaviour and physical activity were objectively measured over an eight day period using the Actigraph GT3X+ accelerometer (Actigraph, Pensacola, FL, USA) worn on the right hip continuously, except during bathing or aquatic activities. Data were recorded at 30 Hz and vertical axis movement counts ‘per 60 second epoch’ used in current analyses. Accelerometer data were downloaded and processed in SAS (version 9.3, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA) [52]. Common thresholds [53] were used to class each minute as sedentary (<100 counts per minute (cpm)), light intensity (100–1951 cpm), moderate intensity (1952–5724 cpm) orvigorous intensity (>5724 cpm). Minutes spent in moderate and vigorous physical activity (MVPA) per day; and sedentary time as percentage of non-MVPA time were calculated for each participant based on their valid days (≥ 10h of waking wear time) [54].

2.12 Statistical analysis

To provide reference values for pain hypersensitivity, quantile regression analyses were conducted to determine the 5th, 10th and 25th for PPT [6], and 95th, 90th and 75th percentiles for CPT, estimated with bootstrapped standard errors (1000 replications). Reference values for pain hyposensitivity were determined using the 75th, 90th and 95th percentiles for PPT and 25th, 10th and 5th for CPT.

Multivariable independent correlates of PPT were determined with linear regression utilising generalised estimating equations, with an exchangeable correlation structure (r = 0.76) to account for the non-independence of the data due to repeated measures by site. Although the PPT was slightly right-skewed, logarithmic transformation did not significantly improve fit of the models and untransformed measures were used to facilitate clinical interpretation of regression coefficients. Multivariable independent correlates of CPT were determined using tobit regression, which was left-censored due to the lower limit of the testing equipment being 5°C. For both PPT and CPT, a series of univariable analyses were initially performed for sex, site (PPT only), rater, ethnicity, height, weight, smoking, activity, quality of life and psychological measures. Sex interactions were tested for all correlates. Sex, and variables with sex-adjusted associations of p <0.15, were entered into multivariable regression models, using a purposeful selection of covariates approach, to ensure no important confounding variables were omitted [55]. Covariates significant at p < 0.05 were retained. Univariable associations were made using all cases available, candidate models were evaluated using cases with available data on all candidate variables, and final multivariable models were estimated using all cases with available data on variables included in final models. Each model was examined to ensure absence of influential observations and collinearity of variables, and linearity of associations and normality and homoscedasticity of residuals. Due to nonlinearity of association with PPT and CPT, BMI was parameterised as underweight, normal, overweight and obese in statistical models. Systematic differences between the 12 research personnel performing the tests in this study were checked. Although some testers’ values were significantly different from others, inclusion of testers in our models confirmed a lack of confounding of the estimates of associations between PPT and covariates by differences between testers [55], and therefore testers were not adjusted for in final models. Stata/IC Version 13.1 for Windows (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX, USA) was used for all analyses.

2.13 Results

The demographic, quality of life, physical and psychological data of the 617 participants are summarised in Table 1. The mean (SD) age of participants was 22.2 (0.6) years with a range of 21.0–24.4, and 280 (45.4%) were female. The SF-12 PCS and MCS mean (SD) values were 55.2 (5.1) and 47.9 (9.6), respectively. Depression, anxiety and stress symptoms were reported as mild by 9.2%, 5.8% and 6.7% and moderate-severe by 14.3%, 12.7% and 8.5%, respectively. The high numbers of subjects with missing activity data (n = 173, 28.0%) represents poorer compliance with wearing the accelerometer. The participants’ number of valid days wearing the accelerometer ranged from 1 to 15 with an average (SD) of 5.3 (2.4).

Table 1

Demographic, quality of life and psychological data summary statistics (n = 617).

Variable Mean (SD) or number (%) Range
Age (years) 22.2 (0.6) 21.0–24.4
Sex (female) 280 (45.4%)
Ethnicity (both parents Caucasian) 525 (85%)
Height (cm)[a] 173 (9.6) 154–201
Weight (kg)[a] 75.9 (17.7) 42.4–147.5
Hip/waist ratio[b] 0.83 (0.07) 0.65–1.08
BMI (kg/m2)[a] 25.2 (5.4) 15.4 –49.3
 Underweight (<18.5) 13 (2.1%) 15.4–18.3
 Normal (18.5–24.9) 374 (60.7%) 18.5–25.0
 Overweight (25.0–29.9) 136 (22.1%) 25.1–30.0
 Obese (<30.0) 93 (15.1%) 30.1–49.3
SF-12[f]
 PCS ≥50 471 (84.7) 24.6–67.3
 MCS ≥50 280 (50.5) 11.7–68.5
Smoking[c] (yes) 90 (14.7%)
DASS-21
 Depression[d] 5.9 (7.7) 0–40
 Anxiety[e] 3.9 (5.0) 0–36
 Stress[e] 7.9 (7.4) 0–42
Activity
 MVPA (minutes/day)[g] 37.5 (28.8) 0–243.2
 Sedentary time (% of non-MVPA during wake time)[g] 63.7 (9.7) 28.6–87.4
  1. BMI:body mass index, DASS-21: depression anxiety stress scale 21, PCS: physical component summary, MCS: mental component summary MVPA: moderate-vigorous physical activity.

References values for PPT stratified by sex and site and CPT stratified by sex are shown in Table 2. For PPT and CPT, reference values for hypersensitivity are reported from the most sensitive to least sensitive, while reference values for hyposensitivity are reported from the least insensitive to most insensitive. For CPT, reference values for hyposensitivity are reported at the 5th, 10th and 25th percentile in females only. Values were unable to be estimated for males, as 38.8% of male participants reached the 5°C minimum cut off.

Table 2

Reference values for pain threshold stratified by sex and site (N = 617).

Pain Threshold (PPT = kPa) Females Males


Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (PPT: P5, P10, P25) Hyposensitivity (PPT: P75, P90, P95) Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (PPT: P5, P10, P25) Hyposensitivity (PPT: P75, P90, P95)
PPT back 275 382 (179) 135, 174, 250 475, 629, 726 334 541 (241) 194, 248, 346 717, 916, 999
PPT leg 277 394 (189) 156, 183, 246 506, 669, 779 334 520 (234) 218, 255, 344 665, 933, 998
PPT neck 277 245 (121) 92, 110, 155 319, 411, 488 334 353 (200) 125, 150, 214 434, 635, 793
PPT wrist 276 360 (144) 162, 194, 256 448, 568, 942 336 492 (217) 197, 244, 340 617, 825, 940
Pain threshold (CPT = °C) Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (CPT: P95, P90, P75) Hyposensitivity (CPT: P25, P10, P5) Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (CPT: P95, P90, P75) Hyposensitivity (CPT: P25, P10, P5)

CPT wrist 274 13.7 (8.3) 26.9, 25.6, 22.0 5.2, 5.0, 5.0 328 10.8 (7.3) 25.7, 23.6, 16.0 Not estimated[a]
  1. CPT = cold pain threshold; PPT = pressure pain threshold; P5 = 5th percentile; P10 = 10th percentile; P25 = 25th percentile; P75 = 75th percentile; P90 = 90th percentile; P95 = 95th percentile.

Univariable and multivariable regression models for PPT are shown in Table 3. Variables with a p-value of <0.15 for univariable association with PPT were site, sex, BMI, WHR, SF12-MCS, smoking, DASS stress and sedentary time. In the multivariable model for PPT measures, site, sex, WHR and SF12-MCS were retained as statistically significant, independent correlates of PPT. The neck was the most sensitive PPT test site (i.e. lowest PPT), and both the neck and the wrist sites were significantly more sensitive than the back and leg PPT test site. The neck was significantly more sensitive than the wrist, and there was no significant difference between the back and leg sites. Males were significantly less sensitive (i.e. higher PPT) than females (mean difference = +141.6 kPa, 95% CI:109.0, 174.3). WHR was associated with increasing pressure pain sensitivity (i.e. lower PPT) with PPT estimated to decrease by 28.2 kPa (95% CI: 5.6–50.8) with each 0.1 increase in WHR. Poorer mental health was associated with increased pressure pain sensitivity (i.e. lower PPT), those with MCS ≥50 were estimated to have 33.0 kPa (95% CI: 2.9–63.0) higher PPT values than those with MCS <50.

Table 3

Univariable and multivariable regression model for pressure pain thresholds (kPa).

Variable Univariable Multivariable


Coefficient (95% CI) p-Value Coefficient (95% CI) p-Value
Site
 Back Ref <0.001[d] Ref
 Leg −4.7 (−15.5, 6.06) 0.390 −5.7 (−19.9, 5.5) 0.321
 Neck −164.8 (−175.6,−154.0) <0.001 −165.2 (−176.4,−154.0) <0.001
 Wrist −36.6 (−47.4,−25.8) <0.001 −36.8 (−48.0,−25.6) <0.001
Sex
 Females Ref Ref
 Males 131.6(103.1, 160.2) <0.001 141.6(109.0, 174.3) <0.001
Ethnicity
 Caucasian Ref
 Non- Caucasian 4.5 (−37.8, 46.7) 0.837
Waist/hip ratio[a] 12.7 (−8.5, 34.0) 0.240[e] −28.2 (−50.8, −5.6) 0.014
BMI
 Under −53.3 (−158.2, 51.5) 0.319
 Normal Ref 0.050[f]
 Overweight 41.6 (4.2, 79.0) 0.029
 Obese −17.6 (−60, −25.5) 0.424
SF12
 PCS ≥50 −2.1 (−5.2,1.1) 0.196
 MCS ≥50 2.2 (0.6, 3.9) 0.007 33.0 (2.9, 63.0) 0.032
Smoke
 No Ref
 Yes 35.3 (−7.1, 77.7) 0.102
DASS21
 Depression −1.2 (−3.2, 0.9) 0.260
 Anxiety −1.1 (−4.3, 2.0) 0.483
 Stress −2.8 (−4.9, −0.7) 0.009
Activity
 MVPA(mins/day)[b] 3.0 (−3.0, 9.0) 0.375
 Sedentary time[c] −1.421 (−3.26, 0.417) 0.130

Univariable and multivariable regression models for CPT are shown in Table 4. Variables with a p-value of <0.15 for univariable association with CPT were sex, SF12-MCS, smoking, DASS stress, DASS depression and sedentary time. In the multivariable model for CPT measures, sex, SF12-MCS and smoking were retained as statistically significant, independent correlates of CPT. Males were significantly less sensitive to cold pain (i.e. CPT was elicited at a lower temperature) than females (difference: −3.5°CC, 95% CI: −5.4, −1.6). Better mental health (MCS≥50) was associated with less sensitivity to cold pain (i.e. lower CPT temperature), those with MCS ≥50 were estimated to have −2.5 °C(95% CI: −4.4, −0.6) lower CPT values than those with MCS <50. Smoking was associated with less sensitivity to cold pain (i.e. lower CPT temperature) with CPT estimated to be 3.5 °C lower (95% CI: −6.5, −0.8) in smokers compared to non-smokers.

Table 4

Non-linear tobit regression table for cold pain threshold (°C).

Variable Univarible Multivariable


Coefficient (95% CI) p-Value Coefficient (95% CI) p-Value
Sex
 Female Ref
 Male −4.2 (−5.9, −2.4) <0.001 −3.5 (−5.4, −1.6) <0.001
Ethnicity
 Caucasian Ref
 Non-caucasian 0.5 (−2.1, 3.0) 0.722
Waist/hip ratio[a] −2.0 (−3.3, −0.7) 0.002
BMI
 Under 1.4 (−4.7,7.6) 0.651
 Normal Ref 0.241[d]
 Overweight −2.3 (−4.5, 0.1) 0.054
 Obese −0.9 (−3.5,1.7) 0.502
SF12
PCS ≥50 0.2 (−0.4, 0.3) 0.120
MCS ≥50 −0.1 (−0.2, 0.0) 0.013 −2.5 (−4.4, −0.6) 0.011
Smoke
 No Ref
 Yes −3.0 (−5.6, −0.4) 0.022 −3.6 (−6.5, −0.8) 0.013
DASS21
 Depression 0.1 (0.0, 0 2) 0.090
 Anxiety 0.0 (−0.2, 0.2) 0.949
 Stress 0.1 (0.0, 0.2) 0.060
Activity
MVPA (mins/day)[b] 0.1 (−0.2, 0.5) 0.462
Sedentary time[c] 0.1 (0.0, 0.2) 0.050
  1. CI: confidence interval, BMI: body mass index, DASS-21: depression anxiety stress scale 21, PCS: physical component summary, MCS: mental component summary, MVPA: moderate-vigorous physical activity, sedentary time = percentage of non-MVPA during wake-time.

3 Discussion

The study has provided reference pressure and cold pain threshold data for a ‘healthy ’ young adult population. The reference values reported here are currently the most comprehensive and robust data available for young adults aged 21−24. A major strength of this study is the consideration of the independent associations of a comprehensive number of potential known correlates of pressure and cold pain sensitivity, findings consistent with those previously reported in response to QST [56, 57, 58].

Comparing our data against current normative reference data highlights the most relevant comparison dataset as Neziri [6] who reported PPT data on 20−49 year olds for the same back and neck sites, using the same approach to data with percentiles for hypersensitivity and hyposensitivity (Table 5 shows a comparison with previously existing age, sex, site, and test specific reference data). In contrast, Magerl [9] using a moving average analysis, had an overall number of participants in each sex group of 90 resulting in small numbers in each of five decadal groups (age range 20−70), and did not report percentile values for hypersensitivity and hyposensitivity. Comparison with Neziri [6] shows our data consistently has a wider range of values and higher standard deviations. For example, the male PPT neck values ranged from 125 (P5) to 793 (P95, SD = 200) compared with Neziri [6] who reported values from 150 (P5) to 451 (P95, SD = 94). This discrepancy may reflect the larger numbers sampled for the current study, or differences in participants due to correlates known or unknown to be associated with pain sensitivity, including geographic location and ethnicity [59]. This largely Caucasian cohort from a southern hemisphere, Mediterranean-style climate may differ in pain sensitivity from other cohorts due to ethnicity, lack of exposure to cold temperatures, sunlight exposure and inflammatory status [60]. Further comparison with other reference data testing similar sites is not appropriate, as these previous studies have included participants from the general population [8, 61], without reporting pain status, thereby complicating interpretation. Including participants with musculoskeletal pain introduces variability into results and limits the post hoc use of data for reference purposes [5]. Screening for pain status here used the question “do you have any current body pain?” Therefore, we cannot exclude that some participants may have experienced pain at another time.

Table 5

Reference value comparison.

Pain Threshold (PPT = kPa) Females Males


Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (PPT: P5) Hyposensitivity (PPT: P95) Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (PPT: P5) Hyposensitivity (PPT: P95)
PPT back
 Waller[a] 275 382 (179) 135 726 334 541 (241) 194 999
 Neziri[b] 75 260 (121) 119 528 75 398 (141) 200 653
PPT Neck
 Waller[a] 277 245 (121) 92 488 334 353 (200) 125 793
 Neziri[b] 75 212 (74) 118 360 75 313 (94) 150 451
PPT wrist/hand
 Waller[a] 276 360 (144) 162 942 336 492 (217) 197 940
 Magerl[c] 90[c] 255 (106) Not reported Not reported 90[c] 263 (175) Not reported Not reported
Pain threshold (CPT = 0C) Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (CPT: P95) Hyposensitivity (CPT: P5) Number Mean (SD) Hypersensitivity (CPT: P95) Hyposensitivity (CPT: P5)

CPT wrist
 Waller[a] 274 13.7 (8.3) 26.9 5.0 328 10.8 (7.3) 25.7 Not estimated[e]
 Magerl[c] 90[d] 15.6 (7.2) Not reported Not reported 90[d] 11.2 (8.2) Not reported Not reported
  1. PPT: pressure pain threshold; CPT: cold pain threshold.

3.1 Pain sensitivity vs. site

The multivariable analysis for PPT measures showed the neck and wrist site to be significantly more sensitive than the back site. Pressure stimulates deep tissue nociceptors [62] and the variation seen in PPT between sites will reflect, at least in part, peripheral nervous system sensitivity that is also influenced by local anatomical variations in density of nociceptors and receptive fields [63]. Neziri [6] did not report testing for site differences for PPT, but comparing their back and neck site mean values suggests a smaller gap than our study, possibly reflecting the smaller sample size. The significant site variations support the need for site specific reference values from large cohorts.

3.2 Pain sensitivity vs. sex

The univariable and multivariable regression models for both PPT and CPT (Tables 3 and 4) show females being significantly more pain sensitive than males supporting the need for sex adjusted reference data. PPT and CPT regression models respectively shows PPT 141.6 kPa lower in females and CPT 3.5°C higher in females. Higher pressure pain sensitivity in females is consistent with previous studies in pain free populations [6, 9, 64], mixed pain status populations [7, 10, 65], and pain populations [66, 67]. Greater sensitivity to cold pain in females is consistent with previous studies in both pain free [6, 9] and pain populations [66, 68]. Systematic reviews have found females are consistently more pain sensitive to experimental pain than males across most pain modalities [19, 69].Studies investigating sex differences in pain sensitivity indicate reasonable evidence that psychological factors, social factors, coping strategies and differences in endogenous pain inhibition might partly explain these differences [70, 71].

3.3 Pain sensitivity vs. BMI and WHR

In the univariable models unadjusted for sex, there was a nonlinear association between BMI categories and PPT, however, this association attenuated when adjusting for sex (p = .577) and BMI categories were not considered for further multivariable modelling, consistent with previous findings from others regarding BMI and PPT [6]. In the current study, no association between CPT and BMI was identified, before or after adjustment for sex (p = 0.241 and 0.506 respectively), in contrast to a previous study [6] which demonstrated an association between less cold pain sensitivity and increasing BMI.

Although there was no univariable significant association between PPT and WHR, when adjusted for sex, higher WHR was significantly associated with increasing pressure pain sensitivity (p = 0.022) and the association remained significant in the multivariable model (Table 3). Conversely, increasing WHR was significantly associated with less cold pain sensitivity in the univariable model, but there was no significant association in the multivariable model. The association between PPT and WHR in this study controls for site differences, sex and psychological status and suggests increasing pain sensitivity as WHR increases. The significant association of WHR but not BMI with sensitivity to pressure maybe due to WHR being a better measure of central adiposity, whereas BMI does not take into account the distribution of body fat [72].There is a strong association between WHR and metabolic risk, independent of BMI, which reflects central obesity [73, 74]. Current evidence also demonstrates central obesity increases levels of circulating pro-inflammatory cells, and this finding suggests shared underlying mechanisms that could modulate both pain sensitivity and metabolic health [75, 76].

3.4 Pain sensitivity vs. smoking

In our study, smoking was associated with less cold, but not pressure, pain sensitivity. In the multivariable model accounting for sex and mental well-being (SF12 MCS), the CPT of smokers was an estimated 3.6 °C lower (i.e.; less sensitive) than non-smokers. Findings from human studies investigating the association of smoking and pain sensitivity have been mixed, probably reflecting small sample sizes, different ages and health status of participants, and mixed methods [8, 77, 78]. Anti-nociceptive properties of nicotine on the central nervous system have been demonstrated [79] and combined with the human studies demonstrate that nicotine can have a significant but variable association with pain sensitivity. This highlights the need to include smoking as a confounder when investigating associations with pain sensitivity.

3.5 Pain sensitivity vs. psychological health

Regarding DASS-21 scores, the only significant univariable association identified was for the stress sub-scale with both PPT and CPT, however neither association was significant after adjustment for sex, in the multivariable model. In multivariable models, better mental wellbeing as measured by the SF12 MCS was associated with less pressure and cold pain sensitivity. Neziri [6] also reported a better SF-36 score was associated with less pain sensitivity. However, a systematic review found the association of depression and anxiety symptoms with pain sensitivity to be ambiguous, consistent with the current findings of different associations with different measures of psychological health [70]. The severity of symptoms may be important, with depressive disorder having an association with increased cold pain sensitivity [22]. Catastrophizing has also been shown to be associated with greater thermal pain sensitivity [68, 80]. While there is some evidence of an association with psychological health and pain sensitivity, it may depend on the measure used, construct considered and level of symptoms.

3.6 Pain sensitivity vs. activity

There was no significant association of MVPA and sedentary time with pain sensitivity. Physical activity has been proposed to elicit exercise induced hypoalgesia via recruitment of endogenous pain modulatory systems [81].The association of activity measured via accelerometry and pain sensitivity has only been investigated in one other small (n = 21) study. Here, evidence was demonstrated in female participants of an association between meeting activity recommendations and lower pain sensitivity, whereas sedentary time had no association [82]. Other studies [83, 84] have used subjective self-report activity questionnaires, with lower pain sensitivity found in participants doing more vigorous activity. Investigations using specific laboratory based exercise interventions have found an immediate association between high level activity and reduced pain sensitivity in healthy participants [24, 85, 86]. Despite a large sample and valid activity measurement, our study did not find significant associations between activity and pain sensitivity. The variables MVPA and sedentary time used here might not be optimal to capture how physical activity is associated with pain sensitivity.

In conclusion, this study currently provides the most comprehensive sex specific reference value data for young adults aged 21−24 years of pressure pain threshold at four body sites and cold pain threshold at the dorsal wrist. This large cohort provides more robust values than existing, smaller studies and further, the population-based, non-clinical cohort provides generalisability and limits participant selection bias. These data will provide an important research resource to enable more accurate profiling and interpretation of pain sensitivity in clinical pain disorders in young adults.The robust and comprehensive data can assist interpretation of future clinical pain studies and provide further insight into the complex associations of pain sensitivity that can be used in future research.

Highlights

  • Provides reference pressure and cold pain threshold data for a ‘healthy’ young adult population.

  • The data represent the most comprehensive and robust data available for young adults aged 21–24.

  • Statistically significant, independent correlates of pain sensitivity measures are provided.

  • The data enable more accurate interpretation of pain sensitivity in clinical pain disorders.

  • Provides insight into the complex associations of pain sensitivity for use in future research.


DOI of refers to article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.09.010.



School of Physiotherapy and Exercise Science, Building 408, Level 3, Curtin University, GPO Box U1987, Perth, Western Australia 6845, Australia. Fax: +161 9266 3699.
http://www.curtin.edu.au
  1. Ethical issues: Ethics approval for the Raine Study Cohort 22 year follow up was obtained from the University of Western Australia (UWA) (RA/4/1/5202).

  2. Author contributions: Significant contributions to this work were made by all authors listed: conception and design (RW, AS, POS, MS, LS), literature review (RW, AS, POS, HS, JMcV, LS), data collection (RW, AS, LS), statistical analysis (RW, AS, HS, LS), writing design (RW, AS, POS, HS, JMcV, LS).

  3. Funding: The 22 year Raine Study follow-up was funded by NHMRC project grants 1027449, 1044840 and 1021855.

  4. Conflict of interest: All authors declare no conflicts of interest (RW, AS, POS, HS, MS, JM, LS).

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the Raine Study participants for their ongoing participation in the study, the Raine Study Team for study coordination and data collection, the University of Western Australia Centre for Sleep Science for utilisation of the facility and the Sleep Study Technicians. We would like to acknowledge the University of Western Australia, the Raine Medical Research Foundation, the University of Western Australia, Faculty of Medicine, Dentistry and Health Sciences, the Telethon Kids Institute, the Women’s and Infant’s Research Foundation, Curtin University and Edith Cowan University for providing funding for the Core Management of the Raine Study. The 22 year Raine Study follow-up was funded by NHMRC project grants 1027449, 1044840 and 1021855.

References

[1] Hübscher M, Moloney N, Leaver A, Rebbeck T, McAuley JH, Refshauge KM. Relationship between quantitative sensory testing and pain or disability in people with spinal pain – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 2013;154:1497–504.Search in Google Scholar

[2] Slater H, Paananen M, Smith A, O’Sullivan P, Briggs AM, Hickey M, Mountain J, Karppinen J, Beales D. Heightened cold pain and pressure pain sensitivity in young female adults with moderate-to-severe menstrual pain. Pain 2015;156:2468–78.Search in Google Scholar

[3] Fuentes CJ, Armijo-Olivo S, Magee DJ, Gross DP. A preliminary investigation into the effects of active interferential current therapy and placebo on pressure pain sensitivity: a random crossover placebo controlled study. Physiotherapy 2011;97:291–301.Search in Google Scholar

[4] Kardouni JR, Shaffer SW, Pidcoe PE, Finucane SD, Cheatham SA, Michener LA. Immediate changes in pressure pain sensitivity after thoracic spinal manipulative therapy in patients with subacromial impingement syndrome: a randomized controlled study. Man Ther 2015;20:540–6.Search in Google Scholar

[5] Gierthmuhlen J, Enax-Krumova E, Attal N, Bouhassira D, Cruccu G, Finnerup N, Haanpaa M, Hansson P, Jensen T, Freynhagen R, Kennedy J, Mainka T, Rice A, Segerdahl M, Sindrup S, Serra J, Tolle T, Treede R-D, Baron R, Maier C. Who is healthy? Aspects to consider when including healthy volunteers in QST-based studies – a consensus statement by the EUROPAIN and NEUROPAIN consortia. Pain 2015;156:2203–11.Search in Google Scholar

[6] Neziri AY, Scaramozzino P, Andersen OK, Dickenson AH, Arendt-Neilsen L, Curatolo M. Reference values of mechanical and thermal pain tests in a pain-free population. Eur J Pain 2011;15:376–83.Search in Google Scholar

[7] Jensen R, Rasmussen BK, Pedersen B, Lous I, Olesen J. Cephalic muscle tenderness and pressure pain threshold in a general population. Pain 1992;48:197–203.Search in Google Scholar

[8] Torgén M, Swerup C. Individual factors and physical work load in relation to sensory thresholds in a middle-aged general population sample. Eur J Appl Physiol 2002;86:418–27.Search in Google Scholar

[9] Magerl W, Krumova EK, Baron R, Tolle T, Treede RD, Maier C. Reference data for quantitative sensory testing (QST): refined stratification for age and a novel method for statistical comparison of group data. Pain 2010;151:598–605.Search in Google Scholar

[10] Lee K-H, Lee M-H, Kim H-S, Kim J-H, Chung S-C. Pressure pain thresholds [PPT] of head and neck muscles in a normal population. J Musculoskelet Pain 1994;2:67–81.Search in Google Scholar

[11] Hafner J, Lee G, Joester J, Lynch M, Barnes EH, Wrigley PJ, Ng K. Thermal quantitative sensory testing: a study of 101 control subjects. J Clin Neurosci 2015;22:588–91.Search in Google Scholar

[12] Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Botefur IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihofner C, Rolko C, Schaub C, Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain 2006;123:231–43.Search in Google Scholar

[13] Leboeuf-Yde C, Kyvik KO. At what age does low back pain become a common problem? A study of 29,424 individuals aged 12–41 years. Spine 1998;23:228–34.Search in Google Scholar

[14] O’Sullivan PB, Beales DJ, Smith AJ, Straker LM. Low back pain in 17 year olds has substantial impact and represents an important public health disorder: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 2012;12:100.Search in Google Scholar

[15] King S, Chambers CT, Huguet A, MacNevin RC, McGrath PJ, Parker L, MacDonald AJ. The epidemiology of chronic pain in children and adolescents revisited: a systematic review. Pain 2011;152:2729–38.Search in Google Scholar

[16] Hoftun GB, Romundstad PR, Zwart J-A, Rygg M. Chronic idiopathic pain in adolescence – high prevalence and disability: the young HUNT study 2008. Pain® 2011;152:2259–66.Search in Google Scholar

[17] Backonja MM, Attal N, Baron R, Bouhassira D, Drangholt M, Dyck PJ, Edwards RR, Freeman R, Gracely R, Haanpaa MH, Hansson P, Hatem SM, Krumova EK, Jensen TS, Maier C, Mick G, Rice AS, Rolke R, Treede R-D, Serra J, Toelle T, Tugnoli V, Walk D, Walalce MS, Ware M, Yarnitsky D, Ziegler D. Value of quantitative sensory testing in neurological and pain disorders: NeuPSIG consensus. Pain 2013;154:1807–19.Search in Google Scholar

[18] Blumenstiel K, Gerhardt A, Rolke R, Bieber C, Tesarz J, Friederich H-C, Eich W, Treede R-D. Quantitative sensory testing profiles in chronic back pain are distinct from those in fibromyalgia. Clin J Pain 2011;27:682–90.Search in Google Scholar

[19] Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, Dion D, Dupuis G, Choinière M. A systematic literature review of 10 years of research on sex/gender and experimental pain perception – part 1: are there really differences between women and men? Pain 2012;153:602–18.Search in Google Scholar

[20] Smith MT, Wickwire EM, Grace EG, Edwards RR, Buenaver LF, Peterson S, Kick B, Haythornthwaite JA. Sleep disorders and their association with laboratory pain sensitivity in temporomandibular joint disorder. Sleep 2009;32: 779–90.Search in Google Scholar

[21] Hennings A, Schwarz M, Riemer S, Stapf TM, Selberdinger VB, Rief W. The influence of physical activity on pain thresholds in patients with depression and multiple somatoform symptoms. Clin J Pain 2012;28:782–9.Search in Google Scholar

[22] Klauenberg S, Maier C, Assion H-J, Hoffmann A, Krumova EK, Magerl W, Scherens A, Treede R-D, Juckel G. Depression and changed pain perception: hints for a central disinhibition mechanism. Pain 2008;140:332–43.Search in Google Scholar

[23] O’Sullivan P, Waller R, Wright A, Gardner J, Johnston R, Payne C, Shannon A, Ware B, Smith A. Sensory characteristics of chronic non-specific low back pain: a subgroup investigation. Man Ther 2014;19:311–8.Search in Google Scholar

[24] Meeus M, Roussel NA, Truijen S, Nijs J. Reduced pressure pain thresholds in response to exercise in chronic fatigue syndrome but not in chronic low back pain: an experimental study. J Rehabil Med 2010;42:884–90.Search in Google Scholar

[25] Vaegter HB, Handberg G, Jørgensen MN, Kinly A, Graven-Nielsen T. Aerobic exercise and cold pressor test induce hypoalgesia in active and inactive men and women. Pain Med 2015;16:923–33.Search in Google Scholar

[26] Girdler SS, Maixner W, Naftel HA, Stewart PW, Moretz RL, Light KC. Cigarette smoking, stress-induced analgesia and pain perception in men and women. Pain 2005;114:372–85.Search in Google Scholar

[27] Nielsen CS, Staud R, Price DD. Individual differences in pain sensitivity: measurement, causation, and consequences. J Pain 2009;10:231–7.Search in Google Scholar

[28] Walton DM, Macdermid JC, Nielson W, Teasell RW, Reese H, Levesque L. Pressure pain threshold testing demonstrates predictive ability in people with acute whiplash. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 2011;41:658–65.Search in Google Scholar

[29] Slater H, Arendt-Nielsen L, Wright A, Graven-Nielsen T. Sensory and motor effects of experimental muscle pain in patients with lateral epicondylalgia and controls with delayed onset muscle soreness. Pain 2005;114:118–30.Search in Google Scholar

[30] Zhou Q, Fillingim RB, Riley Iii JL, Malarkey WB, Verne GN. Central and peripheral hypersensitivity in the irritable bowel syndrome. Pain 2010;148:454–61.Search in Google Scholar

[31] O’Neill S, Kjær P, Graven-Nielsen T, Manniche C, Arendt-Nielsen L. Low pressure pain thresholds are associated with, but does not predispose for, low back pain. Eur Spine J 2011;20:2120–5.Search in Google Scholar

[32] Suokas AK, Walsh DA, McWilliams DF, Condon L, Moreton B, Wylde V, Arendt-Nielsen L, Zhang W. Quantitative sensory testing in painful osteoarthritis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Osteoarthr Cartil 2012;20:1075–85.Search in Google Scholar

[33] Goldsmith R, Wright C, Bell SF, Rushton A. Cold hyperalgesia as a prognostic factor in whiplash associated disorders: a systematic review. Man Ther 2012;17:402–10.Search in Google Scholar

[34] Stone AM, Vicenzino B, Lim ECW, Sterling M. Measures of central hyperexcitability in chronic whiplash associated disorder – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Man Ther 2013;18:111–7.Search in Google Scholar

[35] Coombes BKBPMP, Bisset LP, Vicenzino BP. Thermal hyperalgesia distinguishes those with severe pain and disability in unilateral lateral epicondylalgia. Clin J Pain 2012;28:595–601.Search in Google Scholar

[36] Cruz-Almeida Y, Fillingim RB. Can quantitative sensory testing move us closer to mechanism-based pain management? Pain Med 2014;15:61–72.Search in Google Scholar

[37] Straker LM, Hall GL, Mountain J, Howie EK, White E, McArdle N, Eastwood PR. Rationale, design and methods for the 22 year follow-up of the Western Australian Pregnancy Cohort (Raine) Study. BMC Public Health 2015;15:1–16.Search in Google Scholar

[38] Javanshir K, Ortega-Santiago R, Mohseni-Bandpei MA, Miangolarra-Page JC, Fernandez-de-las-Penas C. Exploration of somatosensory impairments in subjects with mechanical idiopathic neck pain: a preliminary study. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 2010;33:493–9.Search in Google Scholar

[39] Göbel H, Cordes P. Circadian variation of pain sensitivity in pericranial musculature. Headache 1990;30:418–22.Search in Google Scholar

[40] Gröne E, Crispin A, Fleckenstein J, Irnich D, Treede R-D, Lang PM. Test order of quantitative sensory testing facilitates mechanical hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. J Pain 2012;13:73–80.Search in Google Scholar

[41] Wasner GL, Brock JA. Determinants of thermal pain thresholds in normal subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:2389–95.Search in Google Scholar

[42] Walton DM, Macdermid JC, Nielson W, Teasell RW, Reese H, Levesque L. Reliability standard error, and minimum detectable change of clinical pressure pain threshold testing in people with and without acute neck pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 2011;41:644–50.Search in Google Scholar

[43] Geber C, Klein T, Azad S, Birklein F, Gierthmühlen J, Huge V, Lauchart M, Nitzsche D, Stengel M, Valet M, Baron R, Maier C, Tölle T, Treede R-D. Test–retest and interobserver reliability of quantitative sensory testing according to the protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): a multi-centre study. Pain 2011;152:548–56.Search in Google Scholar

[44] Waller R, Straker L, O’Sullivan P, Sterling M, Smith A. Corrigendum to ‘Reliability of pressure pain threshold testing in healthy pain free young adults’ [Scand J. Pain 9 (2015) 38–41]. Scand J Pain 2016;13:17.Search in Google Scholar

[45] Sterling M, Hendrikz J, Kenardy J. Similar factors predict disability and post-traumatic stress disorder trajectories after whiplash injury. Pain 2011;152: 1272–8.Search in Google Scholar

[46] Chien A, Sterling M. Sensory hypoaesthesia is a feature of chronic whiplash but not chronic idiopathic neck pain. Man Ther 2010;15:48–53.Search in Google Scholar

[47] McKenna L, Straker L, Smith A. The inter-tester reliability of anthropometric measurement with portable tools. Eur J Physiother 2013;15:34–41.Search in Google Scholar

[48] Ware J, Kosinski M, Turner-Bowker D, Gendek B. How to score version 2 of the SF-12 Health Survey. Lincoln, RI: Quality Metric Incorporated SF-12v2; 2002.Search in Google Scholar

[49] Ware JE, Kosinski M, Keller SD. A 12-item short-form health survey: construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med Care 1996;34:220–33.Search in Google Scholar

[50] Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol 2005;44:227–39.Search in Google Scholar

[51] Lovibond S, Lovibond P. Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales; 1995.Search in Google Scholar

[52] McVeigh J, Winkler E, Healy G, Slater J, Eastwood P, Straker L. Validity in young adults of automated detection of waking wear from hip-worn accelerometer data with a continuous wear protocol. In: International conference on ambulatory monitoring of physical activity and movement. 2015.Search in Google Scholar

[53] Matthews CE, Chen KY, Freedson PS, Buchowski MS, Beech BM, Pate RR, Troiano RP. Amount of time spent in sedentary behaviors in the United States, 2003–2004. Am J Epidemiol 2008;167:875–81.Search in Google Scholar

[54] Rich C, Geraci M, Griffiths L, Sera F, Dezateux C, Cortina-Borja M. Quality control methods in accelerometer data processing: defining minimum wear time. PLOS ONE 2013;8.Search in Google Scholar

[55] Hosmer D, Lemeshow S, Sturdivant R. Applied logistic regression. New York: Willey; 2000.Search in Google Scholar

[56] Hastie BA, Riley Iii JL, Robinson ME, Glover T, Campbell CM, Staud R, Fillingim RB. Cluster analysis of multiple experimental pain modalities. Pain 2005;116:227–37.Search in Google Scholar

[57] Cruz-Almeida Y, Riley JL, Fillingim RB. Experimental pain phenotype profiles in a racially and ethnically diverse sample of healthy adults. Pain Med (Malden, Mass) 2013;14:1708–18.Search in Google Scholar

[58] Neziri AY, Curatolo M, Nüesch E, Scaramozzino P, Andersen OK, Arendt-Nielsen L, Jüni P. Factor analysis of responses to thermal, electrical, and mechanical painful stimuli supports the importance of multi-modal pain assessment. Pain 2011;152:1146–55.Search in Google Scholar

[59] Rahim-Williams B, Riley JL, Williams AKK, Fillingim RB. A quantitative review of ethnic group differences in experimental pain response: do biology, psychology, and culture matter? Pain Med 2012;13:522–40.Search in Google Scholar

[60] Calton EK, Keane KN, Newsholme P, Soares MJ. The impact of vitamin D levels on inflammatory status: a systematic review of immune cell studies. PLOS ONE 2015;10.Search in Google Scholar

[61] Jensen R. Quantitative sensory testing of patients with long lasting Patellofemoral pain syndrome. Eur J Pain 2007;11:665–76.Search in Google Scholar

[62] Arendt-Nielsen L. Reliability of pressure pain threshold testing (PPT) in healthy pain free young adults. Scand J Pain 2015;9:28–9.Search in Google Scholar

[63] Graven-Nielsen T, Vaegter HB, Finocchietti S, Handberg G, Arendt-Nielsen L. Assessment of musculoskeletal pain sensitivity and temporal summation by cuff pressure algometry: a reliability study. Pain 2015;156:2193–202.Search in Google Scholar

[64] Chesterton LS, Barlas P, Foster NE, Baxter GD, Wright CC. Gender differences in pressure pain threshold in healthy humans. Pain 2003;101:259–66.Search in Google Scholar

[65] Chiu YH, Silman AJ, Macfarlane GJ, Ray D, Gupta A, Dickens C, Morriss R, McBeth J. Poor sleep and depression are independently associated with a reduced pain threshold. Results of a population based study. Pain 2005;115:316–21.Search in Google Scholar

[66] Sterling M, Jull G, Vicenzino B, Kenardy J. Sensory hypersensitivity occurs soon after whiplash injury and is associated with poor recovery. Pain 2003;104:509–17.Search in Google Scholar

[67] Walton DM, Macdermid JC, Nielson W, Teasell RW, Nailer T, Maheu P. A descriptive study of pressure pain threshold at 2 standardized sites in people with acute or subacute neck pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 2011;41:651–7.Search in Google Scholar

[68] George SZ, Wittmer VT, Fillingim RB, Robinson ME. Sex and pain-related psychological variables are associated with thermal pain sensitivity for patients with chronic low back pain. J Pain 2007;8:2–10.Search in Google Scholar

[69] Fillingim RB, King CD, Ribeiro-Dasilva MC, Rahim-Williams B, Riley Iii JL. Sex gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. J Pain 2009;10:447–85.Search in Google Scholar

[70] Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, Dion D, Dupuis G, Choinière M. A systematic literature review of 10 years of research on sex/gender and pain perception – part 2: do biopsychosocial factors alter pain sensitivity differently in women and men? Pain 2012;153:619–35.Search in Google Scholar

[71] Bulls HW, Freeman EL, Anderson AJB, Robbins MT, Ness TJ, Goodin BR. Sex differences in experimental measures of pain sensitivity and endogenous pain inhibition. J Pain Res 2015;8:311–20.Search in Google Scholar

[72] Olivier M. Body fat distribution, lipoprotein metabolism, and insulin resistance: a lifetime of research on the pathophysiology of the human metabolic syndrome. J Clin Lipidol 2012;6:601–3.Search in Google Scholar

[73] Welbron TA, Dhaliwal SS, Bennett SA. Waist-hip ratio is the dominant risk factor predicting cardiovascular death in Australia. Med J Aust 2003;179:580–5.Search in Google Scholar

[74] Kuk JL, Katzmarzyk PT, Nichaman MZ, Church TS, Blair SN, Ross R. Visceral fat is an independent predictor of all-cause mortality in men. Obesity 2006;14:336–41.Search in Google Scholar

[75] Price RC, Asenjo JF, Christou NV, Backman SB, Schweinhardt P. The role of excess subcutaneous fat in pain and sensory sensitivity in obesity. Eur J Pain 2013;17:1316–26.Search in Google Scholar

[76] McVinnie DS. Obesity and pain. Br J Pain 2013;7:163–70.Search in Google Scholar

[77] Pomerleau OF, Turk DC, Fertig JB. The effects of cigarette smoking on pain and anxiety. Addict Behav 1984;9:265–71.Search in Google Scholar

[78] Pauli P, Rau H, Zhuang P, Brody S, Birbaumer N. Effects of smoking on thermal pain threshold in deprived and minimally-deprived habitual smokers. Psychopharmacology 1993;111:472–6.Search in Google Scholar

[79] Shi MDMPHY, Weingarten MDTN, Mantilla MDPDCB, Hooten MDWM, Warner MDDO. Smoking and pain pathophysiology and clinical implications. Anesthesiology 2010;113:977–92.Search in Google Scholar

[80] Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite JA, Sullivan MJ, Fillingim RB. Catastrophizing as a mediator of sex differences in pain: differential effects for daily pain versus laboratory-induced pain. Pain 2004;111:335–41.Search in Google Scholar

[81] Koltyn KF, Brellenthin AG, Cook DB, Sehgal N, Hillard C. Mechanisms of exercise-induced hypoalgesia. J Pain 2014;15:1294–304.Search in Google Scholar

[82] Ellingson LD, Colbert LH, Cook DB. Physical activity is related to pain sensitivity in healthy women. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2012;44:1401–6.Search in Google Scholar

[83] Naugle KM, Riley 3rd JL. Self-reported physical activity predicts pain inhibitory and facilitatory function. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2014;46:622–9.Search in Google Scholar

[84] Tesarz J, Schuster AK, Hartmann M, Gerhardt A, Eich W. Pain perception in athletes compared to normally active controls: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Pain 2012;153:1253–62.Search in Google Scholar

[85] Van Oosterwijck J, Nijs J, Meeus M, Van Loo M, Paul L. Lack of endogenous pain inhibition during exercise in people with chronic whiplash associated disorders: an experimental study. J Pain 2012;13:242–54.Search in Google Scholar

[86] Koltyn KF. Analgesia following exercise: a review. Sports Med 2000;29:85–98.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2016-06-06
Revised: 2016-07-27
Accepted: 2016-08-03
Published Online: 2016-10-01
Published in Print: 2016-10-01

© 2016 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain

Downloaded on 26.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.08.003/html
Scroll to top button