Skip to main content
Log in

Simultaneously Observing Concurrently-Available Schedules as a Means to Study the Near Miss Event in Simulated Slot Machine Gambling

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Traditionally, near-miss events in games of skill provide feedback to an individual regarding his or her performance. However, in games of chance, like slot machine gambling, the use fails to carry over. A near miss in slot machine gambling may still be endorsed when most of the symbols falling on a payline match, though technically this arrangement provides no real measure of skill or progress. To date, attempts to study the near miss in slot machine gambling have used resistance to extinction and preference assessment preparations, both of which unsuccessfully capture any putative reinforcement properties. The current investigation introduces a new methodology to assess putative conditioned reinforcement properties of stimuli correlated with the near miss in simulated slot machine gambling by incorporating the observing response with concurrently available schedules, termed simultaneous observing. Successful tests of the methodology regarding schedule-correlated stimuli in relation to win rates demonstrate its potential use, and failure to identify a near-miss event as producing reinforcing effects for schedule-correlated stimuli adds credibility to its ability to discriminate between functions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Modern slot machines may have over 100 paylines, though the traditional three matching symbols may still be present in the form of scatter symbol wins to initiate bonus games.

  2. Full scripts can be obtained by contacting the primary author.

  3. In the state in which the study was conducted, it is illegal to gamble with one’s own or someone else’s money, or for a cash prize, in an establishment without a gaming license. The procedure used disconnects the gambling outcome from the prize awarded.

References

  • Case, D. A., & Fantino, E. (1981). The delay-reduction hypotheses of conditioned reinforcement and punishment: observing behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 93–108.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Lawrence, A. J., Astley-Jones, F., & Gray, N. (2009). Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry. Neuron, 61, 481–490.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L., Crooks, B., Clarke, R., Aitken, M. R. F., & Dunn, B. D. (2012). Physiological responses to near-miss outcomes and personal control during simulated gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28, 123–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Côté, D., Caron, A., Aubert, J., Desrochers, V., & Ladouceur, R. (2003). Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19, 433–438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daugherty, D., & MacLin, O. H. (2007). Perceptions of luck: near win and near loss experiences. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 1, 123–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLeon, I. G., Bullock, C. E., & Catania, A. C. (2013). Arranging reinforcement contingencies in applied stings: Fundamentals and implications of recent basic and applied research. In G. J. Madden et al. (Eds.), APA handbook of behavior Analysis: Vol. 2. Translating principles into practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillen, J., & Dixon, M. R. (2008). The impact of jackpot and near-miss magnitude on rate and subjective probability of slot machine gamblers. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 2, 121–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R. (2010). The roulette near-miss effect. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 4, 54–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., & Schreiber, J. E. (2004). Near-miss effects on response latencies and win estimations of slot machine players. The Psychological Record, 54, 335–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., MacLin, O. H., & Daugherty, D. (2006). An evaluation of response allocations to concurrently available slot machine simulations. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 232–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., Nastally, B. L., Hahs, A. D., Homer-King, M., & Jackson, J. W. (2009a). Blackjack players demonstrate the near miss effect. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 3, 56–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. R., Nastally, B. L., Jackson, J. E., & Habib, R. (2009b). Altering the near-miss effect in slot machine gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 913–918.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E. (1969). Choice and rate of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 723–730.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E. (2008). Choice, conditioned reinforcement, and the Prius Effect. The Behavior Analyst, 31, 95–111.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., & Moore, J. (1980). Uncertainty reduction, conditioned reinforcement, and observing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 3–13.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., & Romanowich, P. (2007). The effect of conditioned reinforcement rate on choice: a review. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 409–421.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., & Silberberg, A. (2010). Revisiting the role of bad news in maintaining human observing behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 157–170.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., Preston, R. A., & Dunn, R. (1993). Delay reduction: current status. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 159–169.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghezzi, P. M., Wilson, G. R., & Porter, J. C. K. (2006). The near-miss effect in simulated slot machine play. In P. M. Ghezzi, C. A. Lyons, M. R. Dixon, & G. R. Wilson (Eds.), Gambling: Behavior theory, research, and application (pp. 155–170). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gravetter, F. J., & Wallnau, L. B. (2008). Statistics for the behavioral sciences. Belmont: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, M. (1994). The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 351–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Győző, K., & Körmendi, A. (2012). Can we perceive near miss? An empirical study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28, 105–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Habib, R., & Dixon, M. R. (2010). Neurobehavioral evidence for the “near-miss” effect in pathological gamblers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 313–328.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrigan, K. A. (2007). Slot machine structural characteristics: distorted player views of payback percentages. Journal of Gambling Issues, 20, 215–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrigan, K. A. (2008). Slot machine structural characteristics: creating near misses using high award symbol ratios. International Journal of Mental Health, 6, 353–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrigan, K. A. (2009). Slot machines: pursuing responsible gaming practices for virtual reels and near misses. International Journal of Mental Health Addiction, 7, 68–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haw, J. (2008). The relationship between reinforcement and gaming machine choice. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24, 55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hendry, D. P. (1969). Introduction. In D. P. Hendry (Ed.), Conditioned reinforcement. Homewood: The Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, M. (2010). The big book of slots and video poker (2nd ed.). Las Vegas: Cardoza Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the “near miss” and the “big win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 155–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lesieur, H. R., & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1184–1188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLin, O. H., Dixon, M. R., Daugherty, D., & Small, S. L. (2007). Using a computer simulation of three slot machines to investigate a gambler’s preference among varying densities of near-miss alternatives. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 237–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madden, G. J., & Perone, M. (1999). Human sensitivity to concurrent schedules of reinforcement: effects of observing schedule-correlated stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 303–318.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, D. R. (1983). Handbook of slot machine reel strips. Las Vegas: Mead Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nastally, B. L., & Dixon, M. R. (2012). The effect of a brief acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) intervention on the near miss effect in problem gamblers. The Psychological Record, 62, 677–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nastally, B. L., Dixon, M. R., & Jackson, J. W. (2009). The effect of stopping devices and win rate on preference in slot machine players. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 3, 27–30.

  • Near miss. (2012). In Oxford English Dictionary. Retrieved from http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/125544?redirectedFrom=near+miss#eid34963145

  • Peterson, J. M., & Weatherly, J. N. (2011). Comparing three strategies of motivating gambling behavior in the laboratory environment. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 5, 28–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hagopian, L. P., Bowman, L. G., & Toole, L. (1996). Using a choice assessment to predict reinforcer effectiveness. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, R. L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shahan, T. A. (2002). The observing-response procedure: a novel method to study drug-associated conditioned reinforcement. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 10, 3–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shahan, T. A., & Lattal, K. A. (1998). On the functions of the changeover delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 141–160.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shahan, T. A., Podlesnik, C. A., & Jimenez-Gomez, C. (2006). Matching and conditioned reinforcement rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 167–180.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shull, R. L., & Lawrence, P. S. (1998). Reinforcement: Schedule performance. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 95–129). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Silberberg, A., & Fantino, E. (2010). Observing responses: maintained by good news only? Behavioural Processes, 85, 80–82.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1980). Notebooks. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strickland, L. H., & Grote, F. W. (1967). Temporal presentation of winning symbols and slot-machine playing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 10–13.

  • Sundali, J. A., Safford, A. H., & Croson, R. (2012). The impact of near-miss events on betting behavior: an examination of casino rapid roulette play. Judgment and Decision Making, 7, 768–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weatherly, J. N., Thompson, B. J., Hodny, M., & Meier, E. (2009). Choice behavior of nonpathological women playing concurrently available slot machines: effect of changes in payback percentages. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 895–900.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitton, M., & Weatherly, J. N. (2009). The effect of near-miss rate and card control when American Indians and non-Indians gamble in a laboratory situation: the influence of alcohol. American Indian and Alaska Native Mental Health Research, 16(2), 28–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. A. (1994). Conditioned reinforcement: experimental and theoretical issues. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 261–285.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witts, B. N. (2013). Cumulative frequencies of behavior analytic journal publications related to human research on gambling. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 7, 59–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witts, B. N., Ghezzi, P. M., & Weatherly, J. N. (2011). Altering probability discounting in a gambling simulation. Analysis of Gambling Behavior, 5, 83–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyckoff, L. B., Jr. (1952). The role of observing responses in discrimination learning: part I. Psychological Review, 59, 431–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Cleborne Maddux for his invaluable assistance with the statistical tests. We are indebted to the comments offered by the reviewers, and particularly to Reviewer 2’s interesting interpretation. This manuscript was completed as a doctoral dissertation by the first author

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Benjamin N. Witts.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Witts, B.N., Ghezzi, P.M. & Manson, M. Simultaneously Observing Concurrently-Available Schedules as a Means to Study the Near Miss Event in Simulated Slot Machine Gambling. Psychol Rec 65, 115–129 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0095-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0095-y

Keywords

Navigation