Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Drinking for relief: Negative affect increases automatic alcohol motivation in coping-motivated drinkers

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although there is increasing evidence that automatic alcohol motivation plays a role in drinking behavior, little research has examined the contexts that elicit these automatic processes. This study was designed to examine whether negative affect would increase the strength of automatic alcohol-approach associations in individuals who drink to cope with negative emotion. Participants consisted of regular drinkers who were high or low in motivation to consume alcohol to cope with negative emotion. In session 1, participants completed an Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald in, J Pers Soc Psychol 74: 1464–1480, 1998) to assess automatic alcohol-approach associations. In session 2, participants were administered a personalized negative affect imagery task (Sinha in, Imagery script development procedures, version 4.1. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University School of Medicine, 2005) and completed another IAT. The results indicated that the negative affect induction increased the strength of automatic alcohol-approach associations in participants with high coping motivation but not in participants with low coping motivation. These data are the first to document that negative affect can increase the strength of automatic motivational processes related to alcohol.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker, T. B., Morse, E., & Sherman, J. E. (1987). The motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analysis of urges. In C. Rivers (Ed.), The Nebraska symposium on motivation: Alcohol and addictive behavior (Vol. 38, pp. 257–323). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 33–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birch, C. D., Stewart, S. H., Wiers, R. W., Klein, R. M., MacLean, A. D., & Berish, M. J. (2008). The mood-induced activation of implicit alcohol cognition in enhancement and coping motivated drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 33, 565–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2006). Arbitrary metrics in psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 27–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. A., Christiansen, B. A., & Goldman, M. S. (1987). The alcohol expectancy questionnaire: An instrument for the assessment of adolescent and adult alcohol expectancies. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 483–491.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colder, C. R. (2001). Life stress, physiological and subjective indexes of negative emotionality, and coping reasons for drinking: Is there evidence for a self-medication model of alcohol use? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 237–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conrod, P. J., Stewart, S. H., Pihl, R. O., Cote, S., Fontaine, V., & Dongier, M. (2000). Efficacy of brief coping skills interventions that match different personality profiles of female substance abusers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14, 231–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6, 117–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 168–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, W. M., Hogan, L. M., & Kristian, M. R. (2002). Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of abusers’ treatment outcome. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 68, 237–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 12, 163–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2002). The implicit association test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: Strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 115–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2003a). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation (pp. 219–244). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2003b). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50, 77–85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., & De Bruycker, E. (2007). The implicit association test outperforms the extrinsic affective Simon task as an implicit measure of interindividual differences in attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 401–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, R., & Strack, F. (2006). Reflective and impulsive determinants of addictive behavior. In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 45–57). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draine, S. C. (2004). Inquisit 2.0.50401 [Computer software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farris, S. R., & Ostafin, B. D. (2008). Alcohol consumption primes automatic alcohol motivation. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 34, 703–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Francolini, C. M., & Egeth, H. E. (1980). On the nonautomaticity of ‘automatic’ activation: Evidence of selective seeing. Perception and Psychophysics, 27, 331–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, M. S., Brown, S. A., Christiansen, B. A., & Smith, G. T. (1991). Alcoholism and memory: Broadening the scope of alcohol-expectancy research. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 137–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, M. S., Del Boca, F. K., & Darkes, J. (1999). Alcohol expectancy theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In K. E. Leonard & H. T. Blane (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism (2nd ed. ed., pp. 203–246). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, V. V., Stewart, S. H., & Birch, C. D. (2007). Impact of positive and anxious mood on implicit alcohol-related cognitions in internally motivated undergraduate drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 2226–2237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the implicit association test at age 3. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 88–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35, 639–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Rauch, W., & Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 497–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hufford, M. R. (2001). An examination of mood effects on positive alcohol expectancies among undergraduate drinkers. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 593–613.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnsen, B. H., Laberg, J. C., Cox, W. M., Vaksdal, A., & Hugdahl, K. (1994). Alcoholic subjects’ attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8, 111–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B. T., & McMahon, J. (1994). Negative alcohol expectancy predicts post-treatment abstinence survivorship: The whether, when and why of relapse to a first drink. Addiction, 89, 1653–1665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., & Mack, R. (1984). On the cost and benefit of cost and benefit. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 29–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, D. Y. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. In J. B. Sidowski, J. H. Johnson, & E. A. Williams (Eds.), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp. 119–137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Kozak, M. J., Miller, G. A., Levin, D. N., & McLean, A. (1980). Emotional imagery: Conceptual structure and pattern of somatovisceral response. Psychophysiology, 17, 179–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, B. C., & Stacy, A. W. (1993). Alcohol outcome expectancies: Scale construction and predictive utility in higher order confirmatory models. Psychological Assessment, 5, 216–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindgren, K. P., Neighbors, C., Ostafin, B. D., Mullins, P. M., & George, W. H. (2009). Automatic alcohol associations: Gender differences and the malleability of alcohol associations following exposure to a dating scenario. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 70, 583–592.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marlatt, G. A. (1996). Taxonomy of high-risk situations for alcohol relapse: Evolution and development of a cognitive-behavioral model. Addiction, 91, S37–S49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marlatt, G. A., & Ostafin, B. D. (2006). Being mindful of automaticity in addiction: A clinical perspective. In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 489–493). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKay, J. R., Franklin, T. R., & Patapis, N. (2006). Conceptual, methodological, and analytical issues in the study of relapse. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 109–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the implicit association test II: Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oei, T. P. S., & Baldwin, A. R. (1994). Expectancy theory: A two-process model of alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 525–534.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., & Marlatt, G. A. (2008). Surfing the urge: Experiential acceptance moderates the relation between automatic alcohol motivation and hazardous drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27, 426–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., & Palfai, T. P. (2006). Compelled to consume: The implicit association test and automatic alcohol motivation. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20, 322–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., Palfai, T. P., & Wechsler, C. E. (2003). The accessibility of motivational tendencies toward alcohol: Approach, avoidance, and disinhibited drinking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 294–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ostafin, B. D., Marlatt, G. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Drinking without thinking: An implicit measure of alcohol motivation predicts failure to control alcohol use. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 1210–1219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. D. (2003). Alcohol-related motivational tendencies in hazardous drinkers: Assessing implicit response tendencies using the modified-IAT. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1149–1162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palfai, T. P., Monti, P. M., Ostafin, B., & Hutchison, K. (2000). Effects of nicotine deprivation on alcohol-related information processing and drinking behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 96–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 29–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brian Research Reviews, 18, 247–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., Meier, B. P., Zetocha, K. J., & McCaul, K. D. (2005). Smoking and the implicit association test: When the contrast category determines the theoretical conclusions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 201–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., De La Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol use consumption-II. Addiction, 88, 791–804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sayette, M. A., Monti, P. M., Rohsenow, D. J., Gulliver, S. B., Colby, S., Sirota, A., et al. (1994). The effects of cue exposure on reaction time in male alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 629–634.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenmakers, T., Wiers, R. W., & Field, M. (2008). Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers. Psychopharmacology, 197, 169–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, R. (2001). How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology, 158, 343–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, R. (2005). Imagery script development procedures, version 4.1. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University School of Medicine, USA.

  • Sinha, R., Catapano, D., & O’Malley, S. (1999). Stress-induced craving and stress response in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology, 142, 343–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stacy, A. W. (1997). Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 61–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the implicit association test immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stetter, F., Chaluppa, C., Ackerman, K., Straube, E. R., & Mann, K. (1994). Alcoholics’ selective processing of alcohol related words and cognitive performance on a Stroop task. European Journal of Psychiatry, 9, 71–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teachman, B. A., Gregg, A. P., & Woody, S. R. (2001). Implicit associations for fear-relevant stimuli among individuals with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 226–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review, 97, 147–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Widiger, T. A., & Smith, G. T. (1994). Substance use disorder: Abuse, dependence and dyscontrol. Addiction, 89, 267–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., & Stacy, A. W. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., van Woerden, N., Smulders, F. T. Y., & de Jong, P. J. (2002). Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 648–658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Van de Luitgaarden, J., Van den Wildenberg, E., & Smulders, F. T. Y. (2005). Challenging implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in young heavy drinkers. Addiction, 100, 806–819.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wikler, A. (1948). Recent progress in research on the neurophysiological basis of morphine addiction. American Journal of Psychiatry, 105, 329–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wikler, A. (1980). Conditioning processes in opioid dependence and in relapse. In A. Wikler (Ed.), Opioid dependence: Mechanisms and treatment. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zinser, M. C., Baker, T. B., Sherman, J. E., & Cannon, D. S. (1992). Relation between self-reported affect and drug urges and cravings in continuing and withdrawing smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 617–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian D. Ostafin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ostafin, B.D., Brooks, J.J. Drinking for relief: Negative affect increases automatic alcohol motivation in coping-motivated drinkers. Motiv Emot 35, 285–295 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9194-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9194-5

Keywords

Navigation