Skip to main content
Log in

Sibling imitation and contrast effects

  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A general linear model is developed to account for the effects of direct phenotypic imitation and contrast of sibs on one another. Specific predictions from an imitation/contrast model depend upon three assumptions. They are (1) the type of process (imitation or contrast); (2) the stage of the process at the time phenotypes are measured; and (3) sibling constellation variables such as the number, sex composition, and age distribution of a sibling pedigree. For some models, phenotypic variance becomes a polynomial function of the imitation/contrast parameters and the covariances between the genotypes and the environments of sibling pairs. Consequently, phenotypic variances of different types of siblings [e.g., monozygotic (MZ) twins versus dizygotic (DZ) twins versus foster sibs] provide information about imitation or contrast. Thus, standardization of measures prior to analysis may unwittingly hide the statistical information that could detect these effects. The types of data needed to analyze imitation and contrast effects and the potential power of resolving these components of variation are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramovitch, R., Carter, C., and Lando, B. (1979). Sibling interaction in the home.Child Dev. 50:997–1003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., and Kupers, C. J. (1964). Transmission of patterns of self-reinforcement through modelling.J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 69:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., and Walters, R. H. (1963).Social Learning and Personality Development, Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., and Menlove, F. L. (1967). Vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 5:16–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, G., and Rice, J. (1983). Genetics and personality temperament: Simplicity or complexity?Behav. Genet. 13:43–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cicirelli, V. G. (1982). Sibling influence throughout the lifespan. In Lamb, M. E., and Sutton-Smith, B. (eds.),Sibling Relationships: Their Nature and Significance Across the Lifespan, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, N.J., pp. 267–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, C. A., Fulker, D. W., Gurling, H. M. D., and Murray, R. M. (1981). Preliminary findings from a twin study of alcohol use. In Gedda, L., Parisi, P., and Nance, W. E. (eds.),Twin Research 3: Epidemiological and Clinical Studies, Alan R. Liss, New York, pp. 47–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., Rice, J., and Reich, T. (1979). Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. II. A general model of combined polygenic and cultural inheritance.Am. J. Hum. Genet. 31:176–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., and Kendrick, C. (1982).Siblings: Love, Envy, and Understanding. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L. J. (1976). A model for sibling effects in man.Heredity 36:205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L. J., Last, K. A., Martin, N. G., and Jinks, J. L. (1977). A progressive approach to non-additivity and genotype-environment covariance in the analysis of human differences.Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 30:1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L., Last, K. A., Young, P. A., and Martin N. (1978). Model fitting approaches to the analysis of human behavior.Heredity 41:249–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulker, D. W., and DeFries, J. C. (1983). Genetic and environmental transmission in the Colorado Adoption Project: Path analysis.Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 36:175–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, H. H. (1983). Genetic influences on personality from infancy to adulthood.Child Dev. 54:331–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. E., and Mischel, W. (1966). The model's characteristics as determinants of social learning.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 4:211–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, J. L., and Mathews, J. D. (1982). Extensions to multivariate normal models for pedigree analysis.Ann. Hum. Genet. 46:373–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hygge, S., and Ohman, A. (1978). Modelling processes in the acquisition of fears: Vicarious electrodermal conditioning to fear-relevant stimuli.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 36:271–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jinks, J. L., and Fulker, D. W. (1970). A comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of human behavior.Psychol. Bull. 73:311–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. E. (1982). Sibling relationships across the lifespan: An overview and introduction. In Lamb, M. E., and Sutton-Smith, B. (eds.),Sibling Relationships: Their Nature and Significance Across the Lifespan, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, N.J., pp. 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lange, K., Westlake, J., and Spence, M. A. (1976). Extensions to pedigree analysis. III. Variance components by the scoring method.Ann. Hum. Genet. Lond. 39:385–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C. (1978). Heredity-environment analyses of Jenck's IQ correlations.Behav. Genet. 8:415–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C., and Nichols, R. C. (1976).Heredity, Environment and Personality: A Study of 850 Sets of Twins, University of Texas Press, Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C., Horn, J. M., and Willerman, L. (1981). Personality resemblance in adoptive families.Behav. Genet. 11:309–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R. (1984). Siblings: Fellow travelers in coercive family processes.Adv. Study Aggress. 1:173–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., and Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior.Psychol. Bull. 84:309–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, D. C., and Plomin, R. (1981). The importance of nonshared (E1) environmental influences in behavioral development.Dev. Psychol. 17:517–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S., and Grajek, S. (1982). Similarities and differences among siblings. In Lamb, M. E., and Sutton-Smith, S. (eds.),Sibling Relationships: Their Nature and Significance Across the Lifespan, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp. 357–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogler, G. P., and Fulker, D. W. (1983). Familial resemblance for educational attainment.Behav. Genet. 13:341–354.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This work was supported in part by Grants GM-30250, GM-32732, and AG-04954.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carey, G. Sibling imitation and contrast effects. Behav Genet 16, 319–341 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01071314

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01071314

Key Words

Navigation