Skip to main content
Log in

Brainstorming rules as assigned goals: Does brainstorming really improve idea quantity?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In two experiments (n = 264 and 339), I treat brainstorming rules as assigned goals and compare their effectiveness to that of quantity goals as interventions to improve the number of ideas generated by individuals. Controlling for goal commitment, I find that brainstorming rules alone do not convey an advantage over even a vague quantity goal presented alone for enhancing the number of ideas generated in two common tasks. Detailed contrasts revealed that specific, difficult goals were only partially effective on their own, as expected when goal commitment is moderate. However, I find evidence in both studies that brainstorming rules are useful adjuncts to specific, difficult quantity goals. Importantly, their combination was the only consistently effective improvement over both vague quantity goals and brainstorming rules presented alone. I discuss implications for future research adopting a goal-based view of intervention in idea generation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A confusing problem relevant to the present report is that goal setting research using idea generation tasks commonly refers to them as “brainstorming tasks,” but such studies do not provide brainstorming rules to participants (e.g., Locke 1966).

  2. Time for idea generation exercises such as the one used here has varied from one minute (e.g., Locke 1966) on up (e.g., 25 minutes used by Paulus and Dzindolet 1993). I chose 10 min because it seemed to be a reasonable compromise length and was acceptable to instructors who generously allowed data collection in their course sections.

  3. Pro-rating the goal might seem unusual to some, since idea generation is not typically equal across time. However, my purpose was simply to follow Paulus and Dzindolet (1993) in setting the goal at a level that would be very difficult for individuals to hit (without seeming impossible) so that any discontinuation of goal pursuit would not be due to having accomplished the goal. Data show that this was successful.

  4. Supporting the argument that “tell and sell” instructions help participants to avoid seeing specific, difficult goals as impossible, the quantity goal factor (vague versus specific) did not affect commitment in either study. Accordingly, I do not discuss this issue further in the paper.

  5. Table 1 also shows that brainstorming rules led to higher goal commitment in this study. Although not predicted, the finding that goal content affects commitment is also not unprecedented (Seijts et al. 2004). Given the low average commitment in this study, it might be that brainstorming rules simply sounded a little more fun than quantity goals. Because this effect was not replicated in Study 2, I do not discuss it further.

  6. To make the graph using the procedures recommended by Aiken and West (1991), I first recast the analysis in a regression framework. Because regression and ANOVA are related procedures, the results are the same, but I report Table 2 in the ANOVA framework for consistency.

References

  • Aiken, L. A., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, V., & Paulus, P. B. (1996). A simple dynamic model of social factors in group brainstorming. Small Group Research, 27, 91–114. doi:10.1177/1046496496271005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400. doi:10.1037/h0040373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 497–509. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). GPOWER. Bonn: Bonn University Dept. of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerlach, V. S., Schutz, R. E., Baker, R. L., & Mazer, G. E. (1964). Effects of variations in test directions on originality test response. Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 79–83. doi:10.1037/h0048187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyams, N. B., & Graham, W. K. (1984). Effects of goal setting and initiative on individual brainstorming. The Journal of Social Psychology, 123, 283–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., & Alge, B. J. (1999). Goal commitment and the goal setting process: Conceptual clarification and empirical synthesis. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 885–896. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.6.885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., Wright, P. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2001). The assessment of goal commitment: A measurement model meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85, 32–55. doi:10.1006/obhd.2000.2931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larey, T. S., & Paulus, P. B. (1995). Social comparison and goal setting in brainstorming groups. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1579–1596. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb02634.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., Erez, M., & Locke, E. A. (1988). Resolving scientific disputes by the joint design of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Application to the Erez-Latham dispute regarding participation in goal setting. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 753–772. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.73.4.753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Litchfield, R. C. (2008). Brainstorming reconsidered: A goal-based view. Academy of Management Review, 33, 649–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A. (1966). The relationship of intentions to level of performance. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 50, 50–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal-setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., Latham, G. P., & Erez, M. (1988). The determinants of goal commitment. Academy of Management Review, 13, 23–39. doi:10.2307/258352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, B., Johnson, C., & Salas, E. (1991). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: A meta-analytic integration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 12, 3–23. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp1201_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemeth, C. J., Personnaz, B., Personnaz, M., & Goncalo, J. A. (2004). The liberating role of conflict in group creativity: A study in two countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 365–374. doi:10.1002/ejsp.210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborn, A. F. (1957). Applied imagination. New York: Scribner’s.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parnes, S. J. (1963). The deferment-of-judgment principle: A clarification of the literature. Psychological Reports, 12, 521–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, P. B., & Brown, V. R. (2003). Enhancing ideational creativity in groups: Lessons from research on brainstorming. In P. B. Paulus & B. A. Nijstad (Eds.), Group creativity: Innovation through collaboration (pp. 110–136). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, P. B., & Dzindolet, M. T. (1993). Social influence processes in group brainstorming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 575–586. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, P. B., Nakui, T., Putman, V. L., & Brown, V. R. (2006). Effects of task instructions and brief breaks on brainstorming. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10, 206–219. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.10.3.206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. C. (2000). Idea generation in groups: A basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82, 76–87. doi:10.1006/obhd.2000.2888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seijts, G. H., Latham, G. P., Tasa, K., & Latham, B. W. (2004). Goal setting and goal orientation: An integration of two different yet related literatures. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 227–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R. I., & Hargadon, A. (1996). Brainstorming groups in context: Effectiveness in a product design firm. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41, 685–718. doi:10.2307/2393872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegge, J., & Haslam, S. A. (2005). Improving work motivation and performance in brainstorming groups: The effects of three group goal-setting strategies. European Journal of Work and Occupational Psychology, 14, 400–430. doi:10.1080/13594320500349961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowlegments

I thank associate editor Mark Muraven and the anonymous reviewers for their many helpful comments. I thank Angela Henderson for data coding. An earlier version of Study 1 was presented at the 2007 annual meeting of the Academy of Management.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert C. Litchfield.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Litchfield, R.C. Brainstorming rules as assigned goals: Does brainstorming really improve idea quantity?. Motiv Emot 33, 25–31 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9109-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9109-x

Keywords

Navigation