Skip to main content
Log in

Etiological Distinctions between Aggressive and Non-aggressive Antisocial Behavior: Results from a Nuclear Twin Family Model

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A recent meta-analysis of 103 studies Burt (Clinical Psychology Review, 29:163–178, 2009a) highlighted the presence of etiological distinctions between aggressive (AGG) and non-aggressive rule-breaking (RB) dimensions of antisocial behavior, such that AGG was more heritable than was RB, whereas RB was more influenced by the shared environment. Unfortunately, behavioral genetic research on antisocial behavior to date (and thus, the research upon which the meta-analysis was based) has relied almost exclusively on the classical twin model. This reliance is problematic, as the strict assumptions that undergird this model (e.g., shared environmental and dominant genetic influences are not present simultaneously; there is no assortative mating) can have significant consequences on heritability estimates when they are violated. The nuclear twin family model, by contrast, allows researchers to relax and statistically evaluate many of the assumptions of the classical twin design by incorporating parental self-report data along with the more standard twin data. The goal of the current study was thus to evaluate whether prior findings of etiological distinctions between AGG and RB persisted when using the nuclear twin family model. We examined a sample of 312 child twin families from the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Results strongly supported prior findings of etiological distinctions between AGG and RB, such that broad genetic influences were observed to be particularly important to AGG whereas shared environmental influences contributed only to RB. Nevertheless, the current findings also implied that additive genetic influences on antisocial behavior may be overestimated when using the classical twin design.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Crucial to twin methodology is the Equal Environments Assumption (EEA), which assumes that MZ twin pairs are no more likely to share the environmental factors that are etiologically relevant to the phenotype under study than are DZ twin pairs. Under this assumption, any differences in MZ and DZ correlations are due to differences in their genetic similarity. The EEA has been repeatedly tested and found to be valid for numerous phenotypes, including antisocial behavior (as reviewed in Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGruffin, 2008). As such, the EEA is not a focus of the current study.

  2. The full ADSE model was chosen as our full model comparison because 1) neither the ASFE model nor the ADFE model provided a better fit to the data; and 2) the best-fitting models for AGG and RB were the ADE and ASE models, respectively. The ADSE model could thus serve as a basis of comparison across the two phenotypes. This decision was bolstered by the fact that F was estimated to be near zero for both AGG and RB (1 % of the variance or less) and was non-significant. In keeping with this very small estimate of F, there was also little evidence of covariance between A and F. Such findings imply that passive rGE has little, if any, influence on the presence of aggressive or non-aggressive antisocial behaviors during childhood.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2003). Manual for ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allison, P. D. (2003). Missing data techniques in structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 545–557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. A., Barton, M., & Raine, A. (2002). The Southern California Twin Register at the University of Southern California. Twin Research, 5, 456–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, E. D., & Maughan, B. (2009). Differentiating early-onset persistent versus childhood-limited conduct problem youth. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 900–908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A. (2009a). Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 163–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A. (2009b). Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 608–637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A. (2012). How do we optimally conceptualize the heterogeneity within antisocial behavior? An argument for aggressive versus non-aggressive behavioral dimensions. Clinical Psychology Review, doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.006.

  • Burt, S. A., & Donnellan, M. B. (2009). Development and validation of the sub-types of antisocial behavior questionnaire (STAB). Aggressive Behavior, 35, 376–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Evidence that the sub-types of antisocial behavior questionnaire (STAB) predicts momentary reports of acting-out behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 917–920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A., & Klump, K. L. (2009). The etiological moderation of aggressive and non-aggressive antisocial behavior by age. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 343–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2009). Aggressive versus non-aggressive antisocial behavior: Distinctive etiological moderation by age. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1164–1176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Reynolds, S. (1995). Diagnosis and classification of psychopathology: Challenges to the current system and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 121–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Croy, C. D., & Novins, D. K. (2005). Methods for addressing missing data in psychiatric and developmental research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 1230–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Levy, F., & Pearsall-Jones, J. (2002). Recruitment and attrition in twin register studies of childhood behavior: The example of the Austrailian Twin ADHD Project. Twin Research, 5, 324–328.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, A. C., Kender, K. S., Eaves, L. J., & Markell, D. (1985). The resolution of cultural and biological inheritance: Informativeness of different relationships. Behavior Genetics, 15, 439–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudziak, J. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Bartels, M., Rietveld, M. J. H., Rettew, D. C., Derks, E. M. & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Individual differences in aggression: Genetic analyses by age, gender, and informant in 3-, 7-, and 10-year-old Dutch twins. Behavior Genetics, 33, 575–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humbad, M. N., Donnellan, M. B., Iacono, W. G., & Burt, S. A. (2010). The association of marital quality with personality and psychopathology: Results from a very large sample of married couples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 151–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M. C., & Coventry, W. L. (2005). Quantifying and addressing parameter indeterminacy in the classical twin design. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8, 201–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M. C., & Medland, S. E. (2008). Evaluation of the cascade model - a new extended twin family model. Paper presented at the Behavioral Genetics Association, Louisville, KY.

  • Keller, M. C., Medland, S. E., & Duncan, L. E. (2010). Are extended twin family designs worth the trouble? A comparison of the bias, precision, and accurracy of parameters estimated in four twin family models. Behavior Genetics, 40, 377–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 929–937.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klump, K. L., & Burt, S. A. (2006). The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): Genetic, environmental, and neurobiological influences on behavior across development. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 971–977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, R. F., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bleske, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Assortative mating for antisocial behavior: Developmental and methodological implications. Behavior Genetics, 28, 173–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S. R., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 411–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Quay, H. C., Applegate, B., Shaffer, D., Waldman, I., et al. (1998). Validity of DSM-IV subtypes of conduct disorder based on age of onset. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 435–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. J. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A research review and a research agenda. In B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), The causes of conduct disorder and serious juvenile delinquency (pp. 49–75). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical Modeling. VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry, 6th Edition.

  • Neiderhiser, J. M., Reiss, D., Pedersen, N., Lichtenstein, P., Spotts, E. L., Hansson, K., et al. (2004). Genetic and environmental influences on mothering of adolescents: A comparison of two samples. Developmental Psychology, 40, 335–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, H., Van Gestel, S., Vlietinck, R., Derom, C., & Derom, R. (1998). Validation of a telephone zygosity questionnaire in twins of known zygosity. Behavior Genetics, 28(3), 159–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGruffin, P. (2008). Behavioral genetics (5th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sclove, L. S. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 53(3), 333–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & Van Der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B, 64(4), 583–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanger, C., MacDonald, V. V., McConaughy, S. H., & Achenbach, T. M. (1996). Predictors of cross-informant syndromes among children and youths referred for mental health services. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 597–614.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Bartels, M., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Causes of stability of aggression from early childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal genetic analysis in Dutch twins. Behavioral Genetics, 33, 591–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, J. H., Van der Ende, J., Koot, H. M., & Verhulst, F. C. (2000). Predictors of psychopathology in young adults referred to mental health services in childhood or adolescence. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 59–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by R01-MH081813 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or the National Institutes of Health. The authors thank all participating twins and their families for making this work possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. Alexandra Burt.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burt, S.A., Klump, K.L. Etiological Distinctions between Aggressive and Non-aggressive Antisocial Behavior: Results from a Nuclear Twin Family Model. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40, 1059–1071 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9632-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9632-9

Keywords

Navigation