Skip to main content
Log in

Priming Sentence Production in Adolescents and Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyper-Activity Disorder

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theoretical accounts of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) posit a prominent role for problems in response inhibition (Nigg 2006). A key avenue for impulsivity in children with ADHD is inappropriate language expression. In this study, we sought to determine whether poor inhibitory control affects language production in adolescents and adults with ADHD. One hundred and ninety-five participants (13–35 years old; 65% male) were presented with two pictures and a verb, and their task was to form a sentence. If deficits in response inhibition affect language production, then participants with ADHD should be more likely than non-ADHD controls to begin speaking before having formulated a plan that will allow a grammatical continuation. The results showed that the ADHD-combined subtype, in particular, was more likely to produce an ungrammatical sequence. Effects were not moderated by age or gender. These data suggest that response suppression deficits in ADHD adversely affect the basic processes of sentence formation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Achenbach, T. (1991). Manual for the young adult self-report and young adult behavior checklist. Burlington: University of Vermont.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. text rev.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 1, 65–94. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., & Murphy, K. R. (2006). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A clinical workbook-3rd Edition. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Cunningham, C. E., & Karlsson, J. (1983). The speech of hyperactive children and their mothers: comparison with normal children and stimulant drug effects. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 16, 105–110. doi:10.1177/002221948301600209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2002). The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 279–289. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beitchman, J. H., Hood, J., & Inglis, A. (1990). Psychiatric risk in children with speech and language disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 283–296. doi:10.1007/BF00916566.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, C. A., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1985). Girls with attention deficit disorder: a silent minority? A report on behavior and cognitive characteristics. Pediatrics, 76, 801–809.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K., Knee, D., & Tsuang, M. T. (1990). Family-genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM–III attention deficit disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 526–533.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaskey, L. G. (2004). Inhibitory language deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disorder: a candidate shared deficit. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Michigan State University.

  • Bock, J. K. (1987). An effect of the accessibility of word forms on sentence structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 119–137. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(87)90120-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K., & Cutting, J. C. (1992). Regulating mental energy: performance units in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 99–127. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(92)90007-K.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K., & Warren, R. K. (1985). Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation. Cognition, 21, 47–67. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90023-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. E. (1996). Brown attention-deficit disorder scales. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, L. A., Nigg, J. T., & Henderson, J. M. (2006). Attentional versus motor inhibition in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 20, 430–441. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.4.430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christianson, K., & Ferreira, F. (2005). Conceptual accessibility and sentence production in a free word order language (Odawa). Cognition, 98, 105–135. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., Davine, M., Horodezky, N., Lipsett, L., & Isaacson, L. (1992). Unsuspected language impairment in psychiatrically disturbed children: prevalence, language, and behavioral characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 595–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K., Erhardt, D., & Sparrow, E. (1999). Adult ADHD Rating Scales: Technical manual. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B. (1996). Biological correlates of learning and attention: what is relevant to learning disability and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17, 114–119. doi:10.1097/00004703-199604000-00011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dick, F., & Elman, J. L. (2001). The frequency of major sentence types over discourse levels: a corpus analysis. Center for Research in Language Newsletter, 13, 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Seidman, L. J., Mick, E., et al. (2000). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 9–20. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00889-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, F. (1994). Choice of passive voice is affected by verb type and animacy. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 715–736. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, F., & Engelhardt, P. E. (2006). Syntax and Production. In M. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 61–91). Oxford: Elsevier Inc.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Flory, K., Milich, R., Lorch, E. P., Hayden, A. N., Strange, C., & Welsch, R. (2006). Online story comprehension among children with ADHD: which core deficits are involved? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 853–865. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9070-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., et al. (2007). Greater attention problems during childhood predict poorer executive functioning in late adolescents. Psychological Science, 18, 893–900. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01997.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jefferies, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Lui, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescents: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861–863. doi:10.1038/13158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlett, K. W., Pellegrini, D. S., & Connors, C. K. (1987). An investigation of executive processes in the problem solving of attention deficit disordered-hyperactive children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 12, 227–240. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/12.2.227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, E. L., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Applegate, B., & Frick, P. J. (1995). Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: a four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 729–749. doi:10.1007/BF01447474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Javorsky, J. (1996). An examination of youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders and language learning disabilities: a clinical study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 247–258. doi:10.1177/002221949602900303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. R., Miller, J. F., Curtiss, S., & Tallal, P. (1993). Conversations with children who are language impaired: asking questions. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 973–978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Barkley, R., Biederman, J., Conners, C. K., & Demler, O. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 716–723. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp. 163.4.716.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Lee, S. S., & Willcutt, E. G. (2005). Instability of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD from preschool through elementary school. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 896–902. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.896.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., Sanchez, R. P., van den Broek, P., Baer, S., Hooks, K., et al. (2000). Comprehension of televised stories in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and non-referred boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 321–330. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, A. S., Wheeldon, L. R., & Krott, A. (2007). Automaticity and control in language processing. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., Balentine, A., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 463–488. doi:10.1093/clipsy/8.4.463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K., & Barkley, R. A. (1996). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults: comorbidities and adaptive impairments. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 393–401. doi:10.1016/S0010-440X(96)90022-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibtion disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 571–598. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.571.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2006). What causes ADHD? Understanding what goes wrong and why. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puig-Antich, J., & Ryan, N. (1986). The schedule for affective disorders and Schizophrenia for school-age children (Kiddie-SADS)—1986. Pittsburg: Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purvis, K., & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 133–144. doi:10.1023/A:1025731529006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rashid, F. L., Morris, M. K., & Morris, R. (2001). Naming and verbal memory skills in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 829–838. doi:10.1002/jclp. 1052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redmond, S. M. (2004). Conversational profiles of children with ADHD, SLI, and typical development. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18, 107–125. doi:10.1080/02699200310001611612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G. D. (1995). Deficient inhibitory control and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 411–437. doi:10.1007/BF01447206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-aged children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 43, 324–339.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., & Schachar, R. (1996). Executive dysfunction as an underlying mechanism of behavior and language problems in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In J. Beitchman, N. Cohen, M. M. Konstantearas & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavior disorders (pp. 128–155). Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Purvis, K., & Schachar, R. (1993). Narrative abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 103–117. doi:10.1007/BF00910492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tirosh, E., & Cohen, A. (1998). Language deficit with attention-deficit disorder: a prevalent comorbidity. Journal of Child Neurology, 13, 493–497. doi:10.1177/088307389801301005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1997a). Wecshler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1997b). Wecshler intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2001). Wecshler Individual Achievement Test (2nd ed.) (WIAT – II). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, M., Hechtman, L. T., & Weiss, G. (1999). ADHD in adulthood: A guide to current theory, diagnosis, and treatment. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wender, P. H., Wolf, L. E., & Wasserstein, J. (2001). Adults with ADHD: An overview. In J. Wasserstein, L. E. Wolf & F. F. LeFever (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 931. Adult attention deficit disorder: Brain mechanisms and life outcomes (pp. 1–16). New York: New York Ac. of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The wide range achievement test administration manual. Wilmington: Wide Range.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: differences by gender and subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 179–191. doi:10.1177/002221940003300206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. (2000). Etiology of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in a community sample of twins with learning difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 149–159. doi:10.1023/A:1005170730653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Boada, R., Ogline, J. S., Tunick, R. A., Chabildas, N. A., et al. (2001). A comparison of the cognitive deficits in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 110, 157–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, S. S. (1988). Production deficiencies in elicited language but not in the spontaneous verbalizations of hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 657–673. doi:10.1007/BF00913476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul E. Engelhardt.

Additional information

Study protocols were approved by Michigan State University’s Human Research Protection Program, and conformed to APA ethical standards. This research was supported by National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01-MH63146 to Joel T. Nigg and Fernanda Ferreira.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Engelhardt, P.E., Ferreira, F. & Nigg, J.T. Priming Sentence Production in Adolescents and Adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyper-Activity Disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 37, 995–1006 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9323-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9323-3

Keywords

Navigation