Skip to main content
Log in

Sex and Cultural Differences in Spatial Performance Between Japanese and North Americans

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies have suggested that Asians perform better than North Americans on spatial tasks but show smaller sex differences. In this study, we evaluated the relationship between long-term experience with a pictorial written language and spatial performance. It was hypothesized that native Japanese Kanji (a complex pictorial written language) educated adults would show smaller sex differences on spatial tasks than Japanese Americans or North Americans without Kanji education. A total of 80 young healthy participants (20 native Japanese speakers, 20 Japanese Americans-non Japanese speaking, and 40 North Americans-non Japanese speaking) completed the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT), the Mental Rotations Test (MRT), and customized 2D and 3D spatial object location memory tests. As predicted, main effects revealed men performed better on the MRT and RCFT and women performed better on the spatial object location memory tests. Also, as predicted, native Japanese performed better on all tests than the other groups. In contrast to the other groups, native Japanese showed a decreased magnitude of sex differences on aspects of the RCFT (immediate and delayed recall) and no significant sex difference on the efficiency of the strategy used to copy and encode the RCFT figure. This study lends support to the idea that intensive experience over time with a pictorial written language (i.e., Japanese Kanji) may contribute to increased spatial performance on some spatial tasks as well as diminish sex differences in performance on tasks that most resemble Kanji.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bennett-Levy, J. (1984). Determinants of performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test: An analysis, and a new technique for single-case assessment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 109–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bibawi, D., Cherry, B., & Hellige, J. B. (1995). Fluctuations of perceptual memory asymmetry across time in women and men: Effects related to menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 33, 131–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, M. H. (1980). The psychology of the Chinese people. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1989). Homework: A cross-cultural examination. Child Development, 60, 551–561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation and mathematics achievement: A comparative study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian high school students. Child Development, 66, 1215–1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collaer, M. L., & Nelson, J. D. (2002). Large visuospatial sex difference in line judgment: Possible role of attentional factors. Brain and Cognition, 49, 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • d’Ailly, H. H. (1992). Asian mathematics superiority: A search for explanation. Educational Psychologist, 27, 243–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrea, E., & Spiers, M. V. (2005). The effect of familial sinistrality and academic experience on cognition in right-handed women. Neuropsychology, 19, 657–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flaherty, M., & Connolly, M. (1995). Space perception, coordination and a knowledge of Kanji in Japanese and non-Japanese. Psychologia, 38, 229–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsberg, H. C., Choi, Y. E., Lopez, L. S., Netley, R., & Chi, C. Y. (1997). Happy birthday to you: Early mathematical thinking of Asian, South American and U.S. children. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning and teaching mathematics: An international perspective (pp. 163–207). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gitterman, M. R., & Sies, L. F. (1992). Nonbiological determinants of the organization of language in the brain: A comment on Hu, Qiou and Zhong. Brain and Language, 43, 162–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grön, G., Schul, D., Bretschneider, V., Wunderlich, A. P., & Riepe, M. W. (2003). Alike performance during nonverbal episodic learning from diversely imprinted neural networks. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 3112–3120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. C., Alsop, D., Glahn, D., Petty, R., Swanson, C. L., Maldjian, J. A., … Gur, R. E. (2000). An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to a verbal and a spatial task. Brain and Language, 74, 157–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hampstead, B. M., Stringer, A. Y., Stilla, R. F., Amaraneni, A., & Sathian, K. (2011). Where did I put that? Patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment demonstrate widespread reductions in activity during the encoding of ecologically relevant object-location associations. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2349–2361.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heil, M., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Gender differences in math and mental rotation. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2, 190–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iben, M. F. (1991). Attitudes and mathematics. Comparative Education, 27, 135–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isomura, A. J. (2002). Verbal and spatial explicit memory performance among Japanese Americans and European Americans: Gender and ethnic differences. Dissertation Abstracts International, 63-B(6), 3034.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janowsky, J. S., Oviatt, S. K., & Orwoll, E. S. (1994). Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behavioral Neurosciences, 108, 325–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, K., Wüstenberg, T., Heinze, H.-J., Peters, M., & Jäncke, L. (2002). Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2397–2408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R. (1987). The intelligence of the Japanese. Bulletin of British Psychological Society, 30, 69–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R., Hampton, F., & Bingham, R. (1987). Japanese, British, and American adolescents compared for Spearman’s g and for verbal, numerical, and visuospatial abilities. Psychologia, 30, 137–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitrushina, M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagoshi, C. T., & Johnson, R. C. (1987). Cognitive abilities profiles of Caucasian vs Japanese in the Hawaiian Family Study of Cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 581–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure complex: Contribution a l’etude de la perception et de la memoire. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, M., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Postma, A., Kessels, R. P. C., & van Asselen, M. (2008). How the brain remembers and forgets where things are: The neurocognition of object-location memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1339–1345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rahman, Q., Wilson, G. D., & Abrahams, S. (2003). Sexual orientation related differences in spatial memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 376–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathie traumatique. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires & France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, I., Phillips, K., & Silverman, L. K. (1996). Homogeneity of effect sizes for sex across spatial tests and cultures: Implications for hormonal theories. Brain and Cognition, 31, 90–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, T., Rose, M., Weiller, C., & Büchel, C. (2005). Contributions of occipital, parietal and parahippocampal cortex to encoding of object-location associations. Neuropsychologia, 43, 732–743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spiers, M. V., Sakamoto, M., Elliott, R. J., & Baumann, S. (2008). Sex differences in spatial object-location memory in a virtual grocery store. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11, 471–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. M., Chen, C., & Lee, S. (1993). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children: Ten years later. Science, 259, 53–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. M., Lee, S., & Stigler, J. M. (1996). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children. Science, 231, 693–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., & Hiebert, J. (1999). The teaching gap: Best ideas from the world’s teachers for improving education in the classroom. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tottenham, L. S., Saucier, D., Elias, L., & Gutwin, C. (2003). Female advantage for spatial location memory in both static and dynamic environments. Brain and Cognition, 53, 381–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vlachos, F., Andreou, G., & Andreou, E. (2003). Biological and environmental influences in visuospatial abilities. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 339–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Butler, T., Cordero, J., Brake, B., Silbersweig, D., Stern, E., & Imperato-McGinley, J. (2006). The relation between computerized and paper-and-pencil mental rotation tasks: A validation study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 928–939.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X. (1994). Gender differences in spatial, verbal, and mathematical abilities for Chinese students: A cross-cultural comparison. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa.

  • Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1994). Acculturation strategies, psychological adjustment, and sociocultural competence during cross-cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18, 329–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, C. S., Kaplan, E., Casey, M. B., & Hurwitz, I. (1990). Delineation of female performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. Neuropsychology, 4, 117–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witman, N. C., Nohda, N., Lai, M. K., Hashimoto, T., Iijima, Y., Isoda, M., & Hoffer, A. (1997). Mathematics education: A cross-cultural study. Peabody Journal of Education, 72, 215–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maiko Sakamoto.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sakamoto, M., Spiers, M.V. Sex and Cultural Differences in Spatial Performance Between Japanese and North Americans. Arch Sex Behav 43, 483–491 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0232-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0232-8

Keywords

Navigation