Skip to main content
Log in

Statistical modelling of gaze behaviour as categorical time series: what you should watch to save soccer penalties

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research on gaze behaviour in sport has typically reported summary fixation statistics thereby largely ignoring the temporal sequencing of gaze. In the present study on penalty kicking in soccer, our aim was to apply a Markov chain modelling method to eye movement data obtained from goalkeepers. Building on the discrete analysis of gaze employed by Dicks et al. (Atten Percept Psychophys 72(3):706–720, 2010b), we wanted to statistically model the relative probabilities of the goalkeeper’s gaze being directed to different locations throughout the penalty taker’s approach (Dicks et al. in Atten Percept Psychophys 72(3):706–720, 2010b). Examination of gaze behaviours under in situ and video-simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action (Attention, Perception and Psychophysics 72(3), 706–720). The probabilities of fixating anatomical locations of the penalty taker were high under simulated movement response conditions. In contrast, when actually required to intercept kicks, the goalkeepers initially favoured watching the penalty taker’s head but then rapidly shifted focus directly to the ball for approximately the final second prior to foot-ball contact. The increased spatio-temporal demands of in situ interceptive actions over laboratory-based simulated actions lead to different visual search strategies being used. When eye movement data are modelled as time series, it is possible to discern subtle but important behavioural characteristics that are less apparent with discrete summary statistics alone.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

References

  • Abernethy B (1990) Anticipation in squash: differences in advance cue utilization between expert and novice players. J Sport Sci 8:17–34

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik E (1956) Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. 2nd edn. University of California Press, Berkeley

  • Button C, Davids K, Schöllhorn W (2006) Co-ordination profiling of movement systems. In: Davids K, Bennett S, Newell KM (eds) Movement system variability. Human Kinetics, Champaign, pp 133–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Croft J, Button C, Dicks M (2010) Visual strategies of sub-elite cricket batsmen in response to different ball velocities. Hum Mov Sci 29(5):751–763

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dicks M, Button C, Davids K (2009) Representative task designs for the study of perception and action in sport. Int J Sport Psychol Special issue on Skill Acquisition and Sport Performance (40:4):506–524

    Google Scholar 

  • Dicks M, Button C, Davids K (2010a) Availability of advance visual information constrains association-football goalkeeping performance during penalty kicks. Perception 39:1111–1124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dicks M, Button C, Davids K (2010b) Examination of gaze behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action. Atten Percept Psychophys 72(3):706–720

    Google Scholar 

  • Dicks M, Davids K, Button C (2010c) Individual differences in the visual control of intercepting a penalty kick in association football. Hum Mov Sci (in press)

  • Feng G (2006) Eye movements as time-series random variables: a stochastic model of eye movement control in reading. Cogn Syst Res 7:70–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FIFA (2006) Laws of the game 2006. Fédération Internationale de Football Association. http://www.fifa.com/documents/fifa/laws/LOTG2006_e.pdf. Accessed 1 May 2007

  • Gelman AB, Carlin JS, Stern HS, Rubin DB (2004) Bayesian data analysis, 2nd edn. Chapman and Hall/CRC, Boca Raton

    Google Scholar 

  • Glazier PS, Davids K (2009) Constraints on the complete optimization of human motion. Sports Med 39(1):15–28

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goulet C, Bard C, Fleury M (1989) Expertise differences in preparing to return a tennis serve: a visual information processing approach. J Sport Exerc Psychol 11:382–398

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellis E, Katis A (2007) Biomechanical characteristics and determinants if instep soccer kick. J Sports Sci Med 6:154–165

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann DTY, Williams AM, Ward P, Janelle CM (2007) Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: a meta-analysis. J Sport Exerc Psychol 29(4):457–478

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McPherson S, Vickers JN (2004) Cognitive control in motor expertise. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol 2:274–300

    Google Scholar 

  • Michieli I, Medved B, Ristov S (2010) Data series embedding and scale invariant statistics. Hum Mov Sci 29(3):449–463

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morya E, Bigatăo H, Lees A, Ranvaud R (2005) Evolving penalty kick strategies: world cup and club matches 2000–2002. In: Reilly T, Cabri J, Araújo D (eds) Science and football v. Taylor & Francis, London, pp 237–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Panchuk D, Vickers JN (2006) Gaze behaviors of goaltenders under spatial-temporal constraints. Hum Mov Sci 25:733–752

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rein R, Button C, Davids K, Summers J (2010) Investigating coordination in discrete multi-articular movements using cluster analysis. Mot Control 14(2):211–239

    Google Scholar 

  • Ripoll H, Kerlizin Y, Stein J-F, Reine B (1995) Analysis of information processing, decision making, and visual strategies in complex problem solving sport situations. Hum Mov Sci 14(3):325–349

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodrigues ST, Vickers JN, Williams AM (2002) Head, eye and arm coordination in table tennis. J Sports Sci 20:187–200

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, Williams AM, van der Kamp J, Ward P (2002) Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. J Sports Sci 20(3):279–287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, van der Kamp J, Oudejans RRD, Scott MA (2004) Perceptual learning is mastering perceptual degrees of freedom. In: Williams AM, Hodges NJ (eds) Skill acquisition in sport: research, theory and practice. Routledge, Taylor & Francis, London, pp 374–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh GJP, van der Kamp J, Williams AM, Ward P (2005) Anticipation and visual search behaviour in expert soccer goalkeepers. Ergonomics 48(11–14):1686–1697

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singer RN, Williams AM, Frehlich SG, Janelle CM, Radlo SJ, Barba DA, Bouchard LJ (1998) New frontiers in visual search: an exploratory study in live tennis situations. Res Q Exerc Sport 69(3):290–296

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sodergren M, Orihuela-Espina F, Clark J, Darzi A, Yang G-Z (2010) A hidden markov model-based analysis framework using eye-tracking data to characterise re-orientation strategies in minimally invasive surgery. Cogn Process (in press)

  • van der Kamp J, Rivas F, van Doorn H, Savelsbergh G (2008) Ventral and dorsal contributions in visual anticipation in fast ball sports. Int J Sport Psychol 39(2):100–130

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers J (1996) Visual control when aiming at a far target. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:343–354

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers J (2007) Perception, cognition and decision training: the quiet eye in action. Human Kinetics, Champaign

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward P, Williams AM, Bennett S (2002) Visual search and biological motion perception in tennis. Res Q Exerc Sport 73(1):107–112

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson MR, Wood G, Vine SJ (2009) Anxiety, attentional control, and performance impairment in penalty kicks. J Sport Exerc Psychol 31(6):761–775

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Withagen R, van der Kamp J (2010) Towards a new ecological conception of perceptual information: lessons from a developmental systems perspective. Hum Mov Sci 29(1):149–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. Button.

Appendix: General model

Appendix: General model

Models were fitted separately to the data for each experimental condition. For a given condition, we let y tk denote the observed gaze location at time t = 1,2,…,T (T = 54), in trial k = 1,2,…,120. This gaze location was categorised as discussed in the Data Analysis section, but because there were no observations were recorded in the ‘other’ category, the possible values for y tk are 1,2,…,9.

We let π i represent the probability that the gaze was directed at state i at t = 1, for i = 1,2,…,9. The probability of transition of gaze location from state i (at time t) to state j (at time t + 1) is denoted by \( \psi_{ij}^{(t)} \), for t = 1,2,…,T − 1 and i, j = 1,2,…,9. Thus, the vector \( \psi_{i}^{(t)} \) gives the probabilities of transition from state i at time t.

For each condition, we model

$$ y_{1k} \sim {\text{Categorical(}}\pi ) $$
$$ y_{tk} \sim {\text{Categorical}}\left(\psi_{y(t - 1)k}^{(t - 1)} \right) $$

The initial probability vector was given a Dirichlet prior distribution (with α a vector of ones of equal length to π):

$$ \pi \sim {\text{Dirichlet(}}\alpha ) $$

Similarly, each row of the first transition matrix was also given a Dirichlet (α) prior distribution.

Autoregressive smoothing

From the first transition matrix, we calculate (using the ninth column as the reference):

$$ \eta_{i}^{(1)} = m\log it(\psi_{i}^{(1)} ) $$

A first-order autoregressive process is then applied (for t = 2,…,T − 1):

$$ \eta_{i}^{(1)} \sim N(\eta_{i}^{(t - 1)} ,\tau ) $$

A Gamma prior distribution on the precision parameter was used:

$$ \tau \sim {\text{Gamma}}(0.5,0.5) $$

A range of values for the parameters in this prior distribution were tried, and it was found that the results were not sensitive to this choice.

With back-transformation, we get the rest of the transition probabilities:

$$ \psi_{ij}^{(t)} = {\frac{{\exp \left( {\eta_{ij}^{(t)} } \right)}}{{\sum\nolimits_{j = 1}^{9} {\exp \left( {\eta_{ij}^{(t)} } \right)} }}} $$

Summaries

We denote the marginal probabilities by γ it , and these give the probability that the gaze is directed to state i at time t. These were calculated by:

$$ \gamma_{i1} = \pi_{i} $$
$$ \gamma_{it} = \sum\limits_{j = 1}^{9} {\psi_{ji}^{(t - 1)} \gamma_{j(t - 1)} } $$

Fixation probabilities, which were the probability of gaze remaining directed at state i from time t − 1 to time t, were denoted by ω it . They were calculated using:

$$ \omega_{it} = \psi_{ii}^{(t - 1)} \gamma_{i(t - 1)} $$

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Button, C., Dicks, M., Haines, R. et al. Statistical modelling of gaze behaviour as categorical time series: what you should watch to save soccer penalties. Cogn Process 12, 235–244 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-010-0384-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-010-0384-6

Keywords

Navigation