Skip to main content
Log in

A novel method for automatic quantification of psychostimulant-evoked route-tracing stereotypy: application to Mus musculus

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Rationale

Route-tracing stereotypy is a powerful behavioral correlate of striatal function that is difficult to quantify. Measurements of route-tracing stereotypy in an automated, high throughput, easily quantified, and replicable manner would facilitate functional studies of this central nervous system region.

Objective

We examined how t-pattern sequential analysis (Magnusson Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput 32:93–110, 2000) can be used to quantify mouse route-tracing stereotypies. This method reveals patterns by testing whether particular sequences of defined states occur within a specific time interval at a probability greater than chance.

Results

Mouse home-cage locomotor patterns were recorded after psychostimulant administration (GBR 12909, 0, 3, 10, and 30 mg/kg; d-amphetamine, 0, 2.5, 5, and 10 mg/kg). After treatment with GBR 12909, dose-dependent increases in the number of found patterns and overall pattern length and depth were observed. Similar findings were seen after treatment with d-amphetamine up to the dosage where focused stereotypies dominated behavioral response. For both psychostimulants, detected patterns displayed similar morphological features. Pattern sets containing a few frequently repeated patterns of greater length/depth accounted for a greater percentage of overall trial duration in a dose-dependant manner. This finding led to the development of a t-pattern-derived route-tracing stereotypy score. Compared to scores derived by manual observation, these t-pattern-derived route-tracing stereotypy scores yielded similar results with less within-group variability. These findings remained similar after reanalysis with removal of patterns unmatched after human scoring and after normalization of locomotor speeds at low and high ranges.

Conclusions

T-pattern analysis is a versatile and robust pattern detection and quantification algorithm that complements currently available observational phenotyping methods.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aldridge JW, Berridge KC (1998) Coding of serial order by neostriatal neurons: a “natural action” approach to movement sequence. J Neurosci 18:2777–2787

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bäck T (1996) Evolutionary algorithms in theory and practice—evolution strategies, evolutionary programming, genetic algorithms. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman R, Gottman JM (1997) Observing interaction. An introduction to sequential analysis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwick VS, Jones DH, Richter JT, Hicks PB, Young KA (2000) Subthalamic nucleus microinjections of 5-HT2 receptor antagonists suppress stereotypy in rats. Neuroreport 11:267–270

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Borrie A, Jonsson GK, Magnusson MS (2002) Temporal pattern analysis and its applicability in sport: an explanation and exemplar data. J Sports Sci 20:845–852

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Canales JJ, Graybiel AM (2000) A measure of striatal function predicts motor stereotypy. Nat Neurosci 3:377–383

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chartoff EH, Marck BT, Matsumoto AM, Dorsa DM, Palmiter RD (2001) Induction of stereotypy in dopamine-deficient mice requires striatal D1 receptor activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:10451–10456

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper JJ, Nicol CJ (1991) Stereotypic behaviour affects environmental preference in bank voles, Clethrionomys glareolus. Animal Behav 41:971–977

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costall B, Naylor RJ, Olley JE (1972) The substantia nigra and stereotyped behaviour. Eur J Pharmacol 18:95–106

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Creese I, Iversen SD (1973) Blockage of amphetamine induced motor stimulation and stereotypy in the adult rat following neonatal treatment with 6-hydroxydopamine. Brain Res 55:369–382

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cromwell HC, Berridge KC, Drago J, Levine MS (1998) Action sequencing is impaired in D1A-deficient mutant mice. Eur J Neurosci 10:266–2432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drai D, Golani I (2001) SEE: a tool for the visualization and analysis of rodent exploratory behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:409–426

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Drai D, Benjamini Y, Golani I (2000) Statistical discrimination of natural modes of motion in rat exploratory behavior. J Neurosci Methods 96:119–131

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellinwood EH Jr, Balster RL (1974) Rating the behavioral effects of amphetamine. Eur J Pharmacol 28:35–41

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler SC, Birkestrand B, Chen R, Vorontsova E, Zarcone T (2003) Behavioral sensitization to amphetamine in rats: changes in the rhythm of head movements during focused stereotypies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 170:167–177

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler SC, Covington HE 3rd, Miczek KA (2007) Stereotyped and complex motor routines expressed during cocaine self-administration: results from a 24-h binge of unlimited cocaine access in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 192:465–478

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Garner JP (2005) Stereotypies and other abnormal repetitive behaviors: potential impact on validity, reliability, and replicability of scientific outcomes. ILAR J 46:106–117

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Garner JP, Mason GJ (2002) Evidence for a relationship between cage stereotypies and behavioural disinhibition in laboratory rodents. Behav Brain Res 136:83–92

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golani I, Kafkafi N, Drai D (1999) Phenotyping stereotypic behaviour: collective variables, range of variation and predictability. Appl Anim Behav Sci 65:191–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griebel G, Belzung C, Perrault G, Sanger DJ (2000) Differences in anxiety-related behaviours and in sensitivity to diazepam in inbred and outbred strains of mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 148:164–170

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschenhauser K, Frigerio D, Grammer K, Magnusson MS (2002) Monthly patterns of testosterone and behavior in prospective fathers. Horm Behav 42:172–181

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer H, Staude G, Wolf W (2007) A method for locating gradual changes in time series. Biomed Tech (Berl) 52:137–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joyce EM, Iversen SD (1984) Dissociable effects of 6-OHDA-induced lesions of neostriatum on anorexia, locomotor activity and stereotypy: the role of behavioural competition. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 83:363–366

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kafkafi N, Lipkind D, Benjamini Y, Mayo CL, Elmer GI, Golani I (2003) SEE locomotor behavior test discriminates C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mouse inbred strains across laboratories and protocol conditions. Behav Neurosci 117:464–477

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kerepesi A, Jonsson GK, Miklosi A, Topal J, Csanyi V, Magnusson MS (2005) Detection of temporal patterns in dog–human interaction. Behav Processes 70:69–79

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kliethermes CL, Crabbe JC (2006) Genetic independence of mouse measures of some aspects of novelty seeking. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:5018–5023

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Larson J, Quach CN, LeDuc BQ, Nguyen A, Rogers GA, Lynch G (1996) Effects of an AMPA receptor modulator on methamphetamine-induced hyperactivity in rats. Brain Res 738:353–356

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon M, Lyon N, Magnusson MS (1994) The importance of temporal structure in analyzing schizophrenic behavior: some theoretical and diagnostic implications. Schizophr Res 13:45–56

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Magnusson MS (2000) Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: t-patterns and their detection. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput 32:93–110

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Magnusson MS (2004) Theme: powerful tool for detection and analysis of hidden patterns in behavior. Reference Manual, Version 5.0. Noldus Information Technology, Noldus Information Technology

  • Mueller K, Hollingsworth EM, Cross DR (1989) Another look at amphetamine-induced stereotyped locomotor activity in rats using a new statistic to measure locomotor stereotypy. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 97:74–79

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council (1996) Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. National Academy, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicol AU, Kendrick KM, Magnusson MS (2005) Communication within a neural network. In: Anolli L, Duncan S Jr, Magnusson MS, Riva G (eds) The hidden structure of interaction; from neurons to culture patterns. IOS Press, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare, National Institutes of Health (2002) Public health service policy on humane care and use of laboratory animals. Department of Health and Human Services, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohl F, Sillaber I, Binder E, Keck ME, Holsboer F (2001) Differential analysis of behavior and diazepam-induced alterations in C57BL/6N and BALB/c mice using the modified hole board test. J Psychiatr Res 35:147–154

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus MP, Geyer MA (1991) A temporal and spatial scaling hypothesis for the behavioral effects of psychostimulants. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 104:6–16

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus MP, Geyer MA, Gold LH, Mandell AJ (1990) Application of entropy measures derived from the ergodic theory of dynamical systems to rat locomotor behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 87:723–727

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus MP, Dulawa SC, Ralph RJ, Mark AG (1999) Behavioral organization is independent of locomotor activity in 129 and C57 mouse strains. Brain Res 835:27–36

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pogorelov VM, Rodriguiz RM, Insco ML, Caron MG, Wetsel WC (2005) Novelty seeking and stereotypic activation of behavior in mice with disruption of the Dat1 gene. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:1818–1831

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Poirel C, Larouche B (1989) Circadian patterns of basic emotional reactivity and stress related events revisited in mice treated with lithium: behavioral rhythmometric analyses. Chronobiologia 16:229–239

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Powell SB, Newman HA, McDonald TA, Bugenhagen P, Lewis MH (2000) Development of spontaneous stereotyped behavior in deer mice: effects of early and late exposure to complex environment. Dev Psychobiol 37:100–108

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Presti MF, Gibney BC, Lewis MH (2004) Effects of intrastriatal administration of selective dopaminergic ligands on spontaneous stereotypy in mice. Physiol Behav 80:433–439

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Randrup A, Munkvad I (1967) Stereotyped activities produced by amphetamine in several animal species and man. Psychopharmacologia 11:300–310

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rebec GV, Bashore TR (1984) Critical issues in assessing the behavioral effects of amphetamine. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 8:153–159

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rebec GV, White IM, Puotz JK (1997) Responses of neurons in dorsal striatum during amphetamine-induced focused stereotypy. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 130:343–351

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson GS, Moore-Ede MC, Czeisler CA, Dement WC (1985) Circadian rhythms of sleep and wakefulness in mice: analysis using long-term automated recording of sleep. Am J Physiol 248:R320–R330

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saka E, Goodrich C, Harlan P, Madras BK, Graybiel AM (2004) Repetitive behaviors in monkeys are linked to specific striatal activation patterns. J Neurosci 24:7557–7565

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiørring E (1971) Amphetamine induced selective stimulation of certain behaviour items with concurrent inhibition of others in an open-field test with rats. Behaviour 39:1–17

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schiørring E (1979) An open field study of stereotyped locomotor activity in amphetamine-treated rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 66:281–287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szostak C, Jakubovic A, Phillips AG, Fibiger HC (1989) Neurochemical correlates of conditioned circling within localized regions of the striatum. Exp Brain Res 75:430–440

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Toyota H, Dugovic C, Koehl M, Laposky AD, Weber C, Ngo K, Wu Y, Lee DH, Yanai K, Sakurai E, Watanabe T, Liu C, Chen J, Barbier AJ, Turek FW, Fung-Leung WP, Lovenberg TW (2002) Behavioral characterization of mice lacking histamine H(3) receptors. Mol Pharmacol 62:389–397

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Turner CA, Lewis MH, King MA (2003) Environmental enrichment: effects on stereotyped behavior & dendritic morphology. Dev Psychobiol 43:20–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yates JW, Meij JTA, Sullivan JR, Richtand NM, Yu L. (2007) Bimodal effect of amphetamine on motor behaviors in C57BL/6 mice. Neurosci Lett 427:66–70

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Daniel Healy for computer and CVS support, Albert Willemsen and Wilant Van Giessen for their technical advice, Scott Carra and Kevin Yee for their technical assistance, Magnus Magnusson, Ph.D. for fruitful discussions regarding pattern detection algorithms, Charles McCulloch, Ph.D. for discussions regarding biostatistical issues, and Noldus for their generous Theme trial license. This study was supported by grants from the Stephen D. Bechtel Jr. Foundation (SJB, AKS), the Brookdale National Fellowship Program (SJB), NIA-AG026043 (SJB, AKS), NIMH-MH065983 (SJB), and NIMH-MH077128 (LHT).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen J. Bonasera.

Additional information

Stephen J. Bonasera and A. Katrin Schenk contributed equally to this work.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary methodsThe pattern-detection algorithm implemented in Theme allows adjustment of a number of parameters that subtly affect searching. These parameters are:

  1. 1.

    Minimum number of times that a pattern must occur to be counted (minimum occurrences, MO)

  2. 2.

    A significance level (α)

  3. 3.

    A search-level parameter that stops looking for patterns once they reach a certain depth (maximum search levels, MSL)

  4. 4.

    A lumping (LF) factor that joins adjacent patterns if their conditional probability of occurring together is greater than a set percentage

  5. 5.

    A redundancy factor that drops newly detected patterns if they start and stop at the same location as previously detected patterns (fast approximate redundancy reduction, FARR)

  6. 6.

    A fast-free limit (FFL) factor that rejects patterns that occur over longer periods of time, while favoring patterns that occur over shorter periods of time

  7. 7.

    A parameter that excludes highly frequent events (frequent-event exclusion, FEE)

Except where mentioned in the following analysis, these parameters were set as follows. MO = 3; by definition, a given series of events has to occur at least twice before even being considered a pattern. We chose MO to reflect the most agnostic assumptions regarding how many times a given pattern may occur. α = 0.001; we focused on only highly significant patterns. MSL = 99; this value would search patterns of maximum possible depth. LF = 0.90; we chose this value to slightly decrease the total number of patterns identified and thus reduce computational time required for complex data set composition. Supplemental Figure 1 shows the effect of varying this parameter along its entire range on final pattern composition. FARR = 0.90; we also set this parameter to slightly reduce total number of identified patterns and reduce composition time. Supplemental Figure 1 shows the minor effect of varying this parameter along its entire range. FFL = 99 (off value); we turned this parameter off to keep the most agnostic assumptions regarding pattern timing. FEE = 2.5; this parameter was designed to reject event types that are known a priori to be noise-related. In our experiments, this condition was not met. We set this parameter to ensure that no event types were excluded from our data set. These are also the same parameter values recommended by Magnusson (2004) in technical discussions of the software package.

Supplemental Figure 1
figure 6

Varying pattern-detection parameters across their operating range evokes small and consistent changes in overall t-pattern-derived route-tracing stereotypy score. Scores were calculated for a randomly selected trial (mouse received d-amphetamine 2.5 mg/kg) while varying the lumping factor, fast approximate redundancy reduction, and frequent-event exclusion (FEE) parameters across the scope of their recommended ranges. Note a consistent small increase in route-tracing stereotypy score when the lumping factor is set to 1.0 (at this value, no pattern lumping will occur); otherwise, route-tracing stereotypy scores are quite consistent across a variety of FARR and FEE values. The parameter set used for all calculations in this study is highlighted in red (GIF 57.3 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 99.7 kb)

Supplemental Figure 2
figure 7figure 7figure 7

Stereotypic locomotor paths detected through t-pattern analysis have appropriate face validity. Representative pattern compositions for mice treated with vehicle (a) and GBR 12909 30 mg/kg (b). For both a and b, the major pattern types detected in the analysis are color coded within the dendrogram vs time plot and the arena outlines for the individual locomotor activity plots. Within each individual locomotor activity plot, the green asterisk and red square depict the path start and stop points, respectively. Blue arrowheads superimposed on the path trace depict movement direction; the width of these arrowheads are proportional to movement speed (longer arrowheads indicate faster locomotor speeds). The number in each individual locomotor activity plot indicates pattern occurrence within the dendrogram. For instance, in b, the group of paths in the red boxes are all instances of one identified pattern occurring in one trial. These paths all have similar shape except for the ‘offshape’ paths marked with a grey × and a grey dot in the dendrogram vs time plot. To find these ‘offshape’ paths for each trial, a consensus strategy was employed. Individual patterns were kept in the analysis if two or more of the investigators agreed that this pattern occurred at least twice within the trial. All patterns not meeting this criterion were deemed ‘offshape’ and excluded from the analysis. The pie chart represents total trial percentage spent in route-tracing stereotypy. Time in seconds. a Representative analysis for mouse receiving vehicle administration. Route-tracing stereotypy accounts for 13% of total trial duration. By consensus, only locomotor pattern 15 was removed from analysis (GIF 42.2 KB). b Representative analysis for mouse receiving GBR 12909 30 mg/kg administration. Composition detected three major patterns shown in three different colors in the dendrogram vs time plot. For dendrograms in black, the 24 instances where that pattern occurred are shown in the first four rows of locomotor plots. For dendrograms in red, the four instances where that pattern occurred are shown in the fifth row. For dendrograms in blue, the 11 instances where that pattern occurred are shown in the last two rows of locomotor plots. Patterns 31, 32, and 42 (all from red “family”) rejected by consensus. Route-tracing stereotypy accounts for 53% of total trial duration (pie chart inset; GIF 104 KB). c Worst-case pattern composition. In this example, a majority of the detected patterns were not included in the final consensus set. Of the 28 trials analyzed by this method, this trial demonstrated the worst performance (in terms of discarded algorithm-identified patterns). d Comparison of t-pattern route-tracing stereotypy scores before (unfilled bars) and after (filled bars) rejection of patterns not identified by consensus (GIF 82.9 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 1.09 MB)

High-resolution image (EPS 3.45 MB)

High-resolution image (EPS 2.15 MB)

Supplemental Figure 3
figure 8

a GBR 12909 increases home-cage locomotion in a dose-dependent manner. Primary effect of GBR 12909 dosage significant by one-way ANOVA (F3,25 = 19.3165, p < 0.001); individual group comparisons significant by Duncan’s multiple range test except for the vehicle vs GBR 12909 3 mg/kg comparison and the GBR 12909 10 mg/kg vs GBR 12909 30 mg/kg comparison. b Home-cage locomotion after d-amphetamine administration increases in a dose-dependent manner until focused stereotypy dominated the behavioral response. Primary effect of d-amphetamine dosage significant by one-way ANOVA (F3,28 = 20.02, p < 0.001); individual group comparisons significant by Duncan’s multiple range test except for the d-amphetamine 2.5 mg/kg vs d-amphetamine 10 mg/kg comparison and the vehicle vs d-amphetamine 10 mg/kg comparison. Error bars are ±1 standard error (GIF 51.8 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 218 kb)

Supplemental Figure 4
figure 9

Total number of detected patterns and overall pattern length and depth increase in a dose-dependent fashion after treatment with GBR 12909. All detected patterns, including those with only two states, included. a Overall trend significant by one way ANOVA (F 3,26 = 14.75, p < 0.001), all pairwise comparison significance values are p < 0.05 or better by Duncan’s multiple range test except comparison between GBR 12909 10 mg/kg and GBR 12909 30 mg/kg. b Increasing dosage of GBR 12909 increased overall pattern length (F 7,223 = 11.65, p < 0.001) and overall pattern depth (F 6,224 = 25.60, p < 0.001). Furthermore, as demonstrated by the color map, for any pattern length or depth, higher GBR doses were associated with more patterns found at that length/depth compared to lower GBR 12909 doses. c Total number of detected patterns increases after d-amphetamine treatment until response dominated by focused stereotypies (F 3,28 = 5.14, p < 0.0059), all pairwise comparison significance values are p < 0.05 or better by Duncan’s multiple range test except between d-amphetamine 2.5 mg/kg and d-amphetamine 5 mg/kg, and d-amphetamine 2.5 mg/kg and d-amphetamine 10 mg/kg. d Increasing dosage of d-amphetamine increased overall pattern length (F 7,224 = 5.66, p < 0.001) and overall pattern depth (F 5,225 = 13.58, p < 0.001) until response dominated by focused stereotypies. Note semilog axes (GIF 46.8 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 464 kb)

Supplemental Figure 5
figure 10

The number of unique patterns in the t-pattern maximal composition decreases with increased psychostimulant dosage. a GBR 12909; one-way ANOVA on dosage effect is F 3,24 = 5.33 (p < 0.005, pairwise comparisons between vehicle and 30 mg/kg, and 3 and 30 mg/kg groups significant by Duncan’s multiple range test). b d-Amphetamine; one-way ANOVA on dosage effect is F 3,28 = 6.83 (p < 0.0013, pairwise comparisons between vehicle and 10 mg/kg, and vehicle and 30 mg/kg groups significant by Duncan’s multiple range test; GIF 31.2 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 283 kb)

Supplemental Figure 6
figure 11figure 11

Stereotypic locomotor paths evoked by d-amphetamine treatment and detected through t-pattern analysis have appropriate face validity. Representative pattern compositions for mice treated with vehicle (a) and d-amphetamine 10 mg/kg (b). Figure annotations analogous to those described in Supplemental Figure 2. a Representative paths for mouse receiving vehicle administration. Route-tracing stereotypy accounts for 12% of total trial duration. Locomotor patterns 3, 7, 9, and 14 were removed from analysis. Note similarities in overall pattern morphology and route-tracing stereotypy score when compared with vehicle trial from Supplemental Figure 2 (GIF 44.6 KB). b Representative paths for mouse receiving d-amphetamine 10 mg/kg. Composition detected a single major pattern repeated 39 times. Patterns 2 and 38 were removed from analysis. Route-tracing stereotypy accounts for 50% of total trial duration (pie chart inset). By observation, the mouse was observed to enter a phase of continual focused-grooming stereotypies immediately after the completion of the final detected pattern (GIF 144 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 1.24 MB)

High-resolution image (EPS 3.52 MB)

Supplemental Figure 7
figure 12

Route-tracing stereotypy scores are insensitive to locomotor distance. White bars depict route-tracing stereotypy scores calculated for mouse locomotion normalized to that seen in vehicle and GBR 12909 3 mg/kg doses; dark grey bars similarly depict route-tracing stereotypy scores calculated for mouse locomotion normalized to that seen in GBR 12909 10 mg/kg and GBR 12909 30 mg/kg doses. Light grey bars depict unnormalized route-tracing stereotypy scores and are identical to those shown in Fig. 4. Only the primary effect of GBR 12909 dosage was significant (F 3,79 = 78.57, p < 0.0001); the primary effect of locomotor normalization and the interaction between normalization and GBR 12909 dosage did not achieve significance (F 2,79 = 1.6; F6,79 = 1.14, respectively; GIF 27.9 kb).

High-resolution image (EPS 95.3 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bonasera, S.J., Schenk, A.K., Luxenberg, E.J. et al. A novel method for automatic quantification of psychostimulant-evoked route-tracing stereotypy: application to Mus musculus . Psychopharmacology 196, 591–602 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-007-0994-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-007-0994-6

Keywords

Navigation