Skip to main content
Log in

A model of sporting performance constructed from autonomic nervous system responses

  • Published:
European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

Four autonomic nervous system (ANS) variables: skin potential and resistance, skin blood flow and temperature — plus instantaneous heart rate and respiratory frequency — were simultaneously recorded during sporting competitions. The performances of 15 marksmen and 7 archers were analysed by comparing the variations in the six parameters during a concentration phase prior to shooting and during the shooting (or active) phase proper. The results of the performances distinguished two groups of sportsmen: firstly, the best shots and secondly, the worst shots. Comparison of the autonomic responses between the groups showed statistically significant correlations among the ANS variables. This made possible a relationship between performance characteristics and a percepto-effector factor to be defined. More precisely, this factor was defined as the number of correlations of the measured variables in the two phases, concentration and shooting. A positive relationship was demonstrated —the higher the factor, the better the performance. In addition, the stability of the factor, was greater within the best marksmen and archers groups. A model of the temporal change of the percepto-effector factor in both groups was given in three dimensions. This modelling of performance characteristics gave a visual indication of the differences between best/worst performances.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams JA (1971) A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Mot Behav 3:111–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein AS, Taylor KW, Weinstein E (1975) The phasic electrodermal response as a differentiated complex reflecting stimulus significance. Psychophysiology 12:158–169

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrol D, Marzillier TS, Watson F (1980) Heart rate and self report changes accompanying different types of releasing imagery. Behav Res Ther 18:273–279

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety J, Jeannerod M, Germain M, Pastene J (1991) Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort. Behav Brain Res 42:1–6

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis M (1985) Visual imagery and the use of mental practice in the development of motor skills. Can J Appl Sport Sci 10:45–165

    Google Scholar 

  • Deschaumes-Molinaro C, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E (1991) Relationship between mental imagery and sporting performance. Behav Brain Res 45:37–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Deschaumes-Molinaro C, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E (1992) Autonomic nervous system patterns correlate with mental imagery. Physiol Behav 51:1021–1027

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Gusto EL, Bond NW (1979) Imagery and the autonomic nervous system: some methodological issues. Percept Mot Skills 48:427–438

    Google Scholar 

  • Dittmar A (1989) Skin thermal conductivity. A reliable index of skin blood flow and skin hydratation. In: Levêque JL (ed) Cutaneous investigation in health and disease. Dekker, New York: 323–358

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman P, Levenson RW, Friesen WV (1983) Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 221:1208–1210

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah MJ (1984) The neurological basis of mental imagery: a componential analysis. Cognition 18:245–272

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowles DC, Christie MJ, Edelberg R (1981) Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology 18:232–233

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg G (1989) The ability of patients with brain damage to generate neural visual images. Brain 112:305–325

    Google Scholar 

  • Held R (1961) Exposure theory as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. J Nerv Ment Dis 132:2632

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry FM (1960) Increased response latency for complicated movements and a “memory-drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Res Q 31:448–458

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry FM (1974) Constant and variable performance errors within a group of individuals. J Mot Behav 6:149–154

    Google Scholar 

  • von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950) Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwischen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften 37:464–476

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1983) Le cerveau-machine, Fayard. Le temps des sciences Odile Jacob, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele SW, Posner MI (1968) Processing of feedback in rapid movements. J Exper Psychol 77:353–363

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacey JI, Batman DE, Vanlehn R (1953) Autonomic response specificity: an experimental study. Psychosom Med 15:8–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Laszlo JI, Bairstow PJ (1971) Accuracy of movements, peripheral feedback and efference copy. J Mot Behav 3:241–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangina CA (1986) Clinical investigation of a psychophysiological treatment method for learning disabilities. Int J Psychophysiol 4:252–263

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulder G (1973) Methods and limits of psychophysiology. Psychiatr Neurol Neurochir 76:175–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulder G, Mulder LJM (1981) Information processing and cardiovascular control. Psychophysiology 184:392–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Passchier J, Helm-Hylkema H (1981) The effect of stress imagery on arousal and its implications for biofeedback of frontalis muscles. Biofeedback self Regul 63:295–303

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew RW (1966) Acquisition of hierarchical control over the temporal organization of a skill. J Exp Psychol 71:764–771

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew RW (1974) Human perceptual-motor performance. In: Kantowitz VH (ed) Human information processing: tutorials in performance and cognition. Erlbaum, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Rippon G (1990) Individual differences in electrodermal and electroencephalographic asymmetries. Int J Psychophysiol 8:309–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Robin O, Vernet-Maury E, Dittmar H (1987) The ohmic perturbation duration (OPD index), an autonomic index to objectivate anxiety and pain response. Pain 4:428

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA (1975) A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 824:225–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, White JL (1972) Evidence for an error detection mechanism in motor skills: a test of Adam's closed-loop theory. J Mot Behav 4:143–153

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernet-Maury E (1991) The cutaneous bioelectrical signals and their source. Inn Techn Biol Med 121:98–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernet-Maury E, Sicard G, Dittmar A, Deschaumes-Molinaro C (1990) Autonomic nervous system preferential response. Act Nerv Super 32:37–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallin BG, Fagius J (1986) The sympathetic nervous system in man. Aspects derived from microelectrode recordings. TINS 92:63–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilder J (1962) Basimetric approach (law of initial value) to biological rhythms. Ann NY Acad Sci 98:1211–1228

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Caterini, R., Delhomme, G., Dittmar, A. et al. A model of sporting performance constructed from autonomic nervous system responses. Europ. J. Appl. Physiol. 67, 250–255 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00864224

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00864224

Key words

Navigation