Skip to main content

Defective colour vision is a risk factor in driving

  • Chapter
Colour Vision Deficiencies XIII

Part of the book series: Documenta Ophthalmologica Proceedings Series ((DOPS,volume 59))

Abstract

Verriest et al (1980) claimed ‘definite proof that colour-defective drivers do not have more accidents than people with normal colour vision’ by showing that colour-defective drivers were not over-represented in a sample of 2058 male drivers who had caused a traffic accident. In the same paper, however, it is shown that protans have significantly more rear-end collisions and other accidents caused by overlooking signal lights. Deutans had more accidents at traffic lights. The differing conclusions can only be reconciled by assuming that colour-defective drivers have fewer other accidents to compensate for their greater frequency of accidents involving signal lights, and this is inherently improbable.

Verriest et al were incorrect to conclude, from their data, that defective colour vision is not a risk factor in driving, because their sample size was too small and the study lacked a control group. Even so, they found that 8.41% of the accident-causing sample had defective colour vision, somewhat more than the usually-assumed 8%.

The odds ratio for defective colour vision as a risk factor, calculated from comparison of the Verriest et al data with those reported by European investigators, was 1.11 This is not significant, but the confidence interval raises the statistical possibility that the odds ratio may be as high as 1.31.

It is to be expected that drivers with defective colour vision will have difficulty locating and recognizing traffic signals because of their reduced ability to differentiate colours and, in the case of protans, their reduced ability to see red signals. We present data that shows that about 20% of anomalous trichromats and nearly 60% of dichromats admit to difficulty recognizing colours while 10-15% of protans admit to difficulty seeing red signal lights. The accident data of Verriest et al (1980) and also those of Hager (1963), taken with other evidence, lead to the conclusion that defective colour vision is a risk factor for driving. However, it is a risk that bears only on the 8% of drivers who have defective colour vision and the number of accidents due to defective colour vision will therefore be relatively small, probably no more than 4 in 1000 accidents. Nevertheless it would be in the public interest to apply a colour vision standard at least for drivers of commercial and public transport vehicles, since they drive greater distances than private drivers and the social cost of accidents involving commercial and public transport vehicles is higher.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bureau of Transport and Communication Economics (1992). Social cost of transport accidents in Australia. Report 79. Aust. Govt. Pub. Service., Canberra

    Google Scholar 

  • CIE (1980). Light signals form rodtraffic control. CIE Publication 48, CIE Central Bureau, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole B.L. and Vingrys A.J. (1983). Do protanomals have difficulty seeing red lights? Proc. CIE 20th Session, Amsterdam, 1983. CIE: 1-3. CIE Central Bureau, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Crone R.A. (1968). Incidence of known and unknown colour vision defects. Ophthalmologica. 155:37–55

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Francois J. Verriest G. Mortier V. and Vanderdonck R. (1957). De la fréquence des dyschromatopsies congenitales chez l’homme. Ann. Oculist., Paris. 190: 5–16. Cited in Crone (1968)

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gramberg-Danielsen B. (1961). Untersuchungen über die Unfallhäufigkeit von Farbenuntüchigen im Strassenverkehr. Klin. Mb1. Augenheilk. 139: 677–681

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hager G. (1963). Das Sehorgan und das Unfallgeschehen im Strassenverkehr. Klin. Mbl. Augenheilk. 142: 427–433

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kherumian R. and Pickford R.W. (1959). Hérédité et fréquence des dyschromatopsies: p. 109. Vigot Fréres, Paris. Cited in Crone (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Koliopoulos J., Iordanides P., Palimeris G. and Chimonidou E. (1976). Data concerning colour vision deficiencies amongst 29,985 young Greeks. Mod. Probl. Ophthal. 17: 161–164

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mann I. and Turner C. (1956). Color vision in native races in Australasia. Am. J. Ophthal. 41: 797–800

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nathan J., Henry G.H. and Cole B.L. (1964). Recognition of colored road traffic light signals by normal and color-vision-defective observers. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 54: 1041–1045

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson J.H. (1938). Anomalous trichromatism and its relation to normal trichromatism. Proc. Phys. Soc. (Lond.) 50: 661–702

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman L.G. (1960). Medical aspects of road safety. Lancet i: 1039–1045

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins H.G. and Bailey I.L. (1975). The organisation and findings of a school vision screening service. Aust. J. Optom. 58: 392

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachsenweger R. and Nothaass E. (1961). Eine Analyse von 4011 Verkehrsunfällen aus augenärztlicher Sicht. Dts.Gesundh. Wed. 12: 567–872

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt I. (1936). Ergebnis einer Massenuntersuchung des Farbensinns mit dem Anomaloscop. Z. Babnärzte. 2: 1–10 Cited in Crone (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Steward J.M. and Cole B.L. (1989a). Incidence of congenital colour vision defects in an Australian optometric population. In: Drum B. and Verriest G. (eds), Colour Vision Deficiencies IX: 109–111. Kluwer, Dordrecht

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Steward J.M. and Cole B.L. (1989b). What do color vision defectives say about everyday tasks? Optom. Vis. Sci. 66: 288–295

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon P and Straker A. (1943). Distribution of colour blind men in Great Britain. Nature, Lond. 152: 690

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verriest G., Neubauer O., Marré M. and Uvijls A. (1980). New investigations concerning the relationships between congenital colour vision defects and road traffic security. Int. Ophthal. 2: 87–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Planta P. (1928). Die Häufigheit der angeborenen Farbensinnstörungen bei Knaben und Mädchen und ihre Feststellung durch die üblichen klinischen Proben. Graefes Arch. Ophthal. 120: 253–281. Cited in Crone (1968)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waaler G.H.M. (1927). Über die Erblichkeitsverhältnisse der verschiedenen Arten von angebor-ener Rot-Grün-Blindheit. Acta Ophthal., Kbh. 5: 309–347. Cited in Crone (1968)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zehnder E. (1971). Die Bewahrung farbensinngestorter Motorfahrzeuglenker im Verkehr. Schweiz. Med. Wochenshr. 101: 530–537

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

C. R. Cavonius

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cole, B.L., Maddocks, J.D. (1997). Defective colour vision is a risk factor in driving. In: Cavonius, C.R. (eds) Colour Vision Deficiencies XIII. Documenta Ophthalmologica Proceedings Series, vol 59. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-5408-6_54

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-5408-6_54

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-6275-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-011-5408-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics