The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

Abstract

Objective: Previous findings of excess brain lactate and delayed end-tidal CO 2 (pCO 2 ) recovery in subjects with panic disorder during hyperventilation suggested altered acid-base regulation. Two models were posited to explain these results: 1) subjects with panic disorder demonstrate greater alkalosis to hyperventilation, implicating increased lactate as directly compensatory, or 2) subjects with panic disorder demonstrate reduced or blunted alkalosis, implicating increased lactate as overly compensatory to a normal pH response. In both models, delayed pCO 2 recovery in subjects with panic disorder could reflect slower pH normalization in the recovery phase. Method: Asymptomatic medicated patients with panic disorder were studied during regulated hyperventilation. Phosphorous spectroscopy was used to measure brain pH every 2 minutes. Nine subjects with panic disorder were compared to 11 healthy subjects at baseline (five scans), during regulated hyperventilation (five scans), and across recovery (10 scans). Anxiety symptoms were assessed with standard ratings. Results: No subject had a panic attack before hyperventilation. Subjects with panic disorder had lower pCO 2 during hyperventilation and slower pCO 2 recovery across the posthyperventilation interval. Despite this different respiratory response in the panic disorder group, brain pH increases were not significantly greater during hyperventilation, nor was pH return to baseline slowed during posthyperventilation. A linear regression model derived from data of healthy subjects showed pH blunting in the panic disorder group. Conclusions: Although subjects with panic disorder had greater hypocapnea during hyperventilation, their observed pH response, not altered from comparison levels, implicated exaggerated buffering. It is suggested that increased lactate could account for these findings.

Research to date strongly suggests the influence of biological factors in panic disorder (14) . There is evidence that the disorder is familial and may be genetically transmitted (5 , 6) . Individuals with panic disorder are susceptible to having panic attacks in response to various metabolic challenges, ranging from sodium lactate infusion (79) , caffeine ingestion (10) , and carbon dioxide (CO 2 ) inhalation (11 , 12) to sustained hyperventilation (13) . In subjects with panic disorder, sustained hyperventilation commonly elicits slower end-tidal CO 2 (pCO 2 ) recovery after cessation (3 , 14) . From several studies, respiratory dysregulation also persists following treatment (15 , 16) , implicating “trait” respiratory components to the disease.

During alkalotic challenges, such as hyperventilation, a number of studies have investigated peripheral blood lactate response. In most investigations, blood plasma lactate abnormally increases in subjects with panic disorder to these challenges, even when there is control for subject fitness (13) or glucose availability (17) . However, the time course and magnitude of acid-base response are different for the peripheral (blood) and central (brain) compartments (18) . In prior work with single-voxel hydrogen ( 1 H) magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) to measure brain lactate response during regulated hyperventilation (20 mm Hg) (19) , more rapid and prolonged brain lactate increases were observed in asymptomatic medication-treated subjects with panic, a group specifically studied to evaluate the underlying features of the disorder. This evidence for abnormal compensation during alkalosis and increasing discussion of acid-base systems in panic disorder (20) led to the current investigation aimed at characterizing brain pH response to hyperventilation directly.

Phosphorous ( 31 P) MRS provides a noninvasive measure of intracellular brain pH derived from the frequency shift between metabolites, most commonly inorganic phosphate relative to phosphocreatine (21) . To improve time-resolution while ensuring a sufficient 31 P signal-to-noise ratio, a larger volume of interest (a 30-mm slab) was employed compared to our single-voxel 1 H MRS work (19) . The diffuse brain lactate response to hyperventilation (22) and the homogeneous pH distribution in the brain (23) suggest MRS voxel localization need not be markedly specific to assess this regulatory system. Two metabolic models in panic disorder were posited to explain the previously observed excess lactate increase to hyperventilation challenge: 1) subjects with panic disorder would demonstrate greater alkalosis to hyperventilation, implicating lactate as directly compensatory to an exaggerated brain pH response; 2) subjects with panic disorder would demonstrate reduced alkalosis, implicating lactate as overly compensatory to a normal pH response. In both models, we hypothesized that delayed pCO 2 recovery in subjects with panic disorder, if present, would be related to slower pH normalization in the posthyperventilation phase.

Method

Subjects

Nine asymptomatic, medication-treated subjects with panic disorder (five men, four women; mean age=42.7 years, SD=15.3) and 11 healthy comparison subjects (three men, eight women; mean age=31.5 years, SD=9.6) were studied using 31 P MRS during hyperventilation. One additional comparison subject was studied but was excluded from analyses because of poor-quality MRS data. Panic disorder diagnosis, based on DSM-IV criteria, was established using a structured psychiatric interview developed at the University of Washington, consistent with prior work (24 , 25) . The comparison subjects were similarly evaluated and had no history of panic attacks or a family history of panic disorder in first-degree relatives, as assessed by subject reports. All subjects were free of other axis I disorders. All subjects gave written informed consent for participation in the study, which was approved by the University of Washington Human Subjects Review Committee. Eight subjects with panic disorder were under treatment with fluoxetine (20–40 mg/day), and one subject was under treatment with gabapentin (3600 mg/day). The subjects with panic disorder were symptom free for at least 12 weeks before a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) examination. While unmedicated, eight of the nine subjects with panic disorder had previously undergone a lactate infusion challenge and experienced a positive panic response, demonstrating group sensitivity to this alkalotic perturbation.

Symptom Assessment

Before and after completion of the MRI procedure, the subjects were administered the Acute Panic Inventory (26) (range=0–51). At similar time points, the subjects were also asked to rate their anxiety on a scale of 0 (none) to 4 (severe) and panic severity on a scale of 0 (none) to 10 (severe) with analog scales.

MR Hyperventilation Procedures

The MRI protocol consisted of a 12-minute baseline period (five scans of 2 minutes, 20 seconds each) in which subjects were instructed to “relax and breathe normally.” After this phase was a 12-minute hyperventilation period (five scans), during which subjects were instructed to breathe rapidly while fully inhaling and exhaling. A nasal cannula was used to sample expired CO 2 (pCO 2 ) throughout the experiment; tubing was run through a conduit in the magnet room and pCO 2 was analyzed with a NellCor 1000 monitor (Nellcor, Pleasanton, Calif.). Before entering the scanner, the subjects were trained to exhale through their noses (ensuring cannula pCO 2 measurement) and to inhale and exhale fully during the hyperventilation period. This latter instruction was aimed at avoiding shallow breathing during hyperventilation, which, by primarily clearing the dead space in the lungs, may affect measurement of pCO 2 . Hyperventilation duration and instructions were similar to a respiratory study that induced a marked pCO 2 recovery delay in patients with panic disorder (14) . During the hyperventilation phase, frequent feedback for all subjects at 30-second intervals was provided to aid in the maintenance of end-tidal CO 2 near 20 mm Hg. At the end of the hyperventilation period, the subjects were instructed to resume normal breathing without further feedback during a 24-minute (10-scan) recovery period.

MRI/MRS Procedures

31 P MRS studies were performed with a clinical 1.5-T General Electric Signa (Milwaukee) whole-body scanner equipped with version 5.8 Genesis operating software. An experimental dual-tuned 31 P- 1 H birdcage coil, adapted from a modified circuit board and based on published work (27) , was built at the University of Washington and used for all studies. The coil was optimized for both 31 P and 1 H after substantial bench tuning.

Proton imaging was performed for detailed localization of the phosphorous MRS axial slab (sagittal T 1 localizer and an axial dual-echo sequence, TE=13/104 msec, 22 cm field of view, and 3 mm slice thickness). An axial section just inferior to the tops of the lateral ventricles was selected as the midpoint of the 30 mm slab examined with 31 P MRS. This anatomical location was chosen to include the anatomical loci used in prior work (19) and to ensure optimal field homogeneity by avoiding sinus inclusion. Furthermore, this large sample volume included minimal signal contribution from nonbrain signals (28) . Before switching to the 31 P section of the coil, two passes of 1 H gradient shimming were conducted to optimize field homogeneity over the axial section.

For 31 P MRS data collection, a spin-echo sequence detailed by Lim et al. (29) was used, employing a TE of 6 msec, a TR of 2 seconds, and 64 scan averages (2 minutes, 20 seconds of acquisition). After acquisition, 31 P data were transferred to a workstation and converted to the magnetic resonance user interface (MRUI) (30) format. For each subject, the first spectrum in the series was used to establish starting parameters for subsequent spectra. Prior knowledge of peak line widths, frequencies, and couplings was included in the variable projection time-domain fitting of Lorentzian lines performed in MRUI with the Bolstad algorithm ( Figure 1 ). The phosphate-phosphocreatine shift was used to compute pH in MRUI (pH=6.75 + [log 10 ([phosphate-phosphocreatine] – 3.27)/(5.63 – [phosphate-phosphocreatine]), similar to the formula described by Petroff et al. (21) . At each scan time point, pCO 2 measures were sampled. Data (pH and pCO 2 as within-subject factors) were compared with a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), often referred to as a doubly multivariate model, by using SPSS 10 (SPSS, Chicago). To assess other metabolite changes over time, fitted amplitudes of phosphodiesters, phosphomonoesters, phosphate, and the high-energy phosphates phosphocreatine and ATP were referenced to the total 31 P signal and also analyzed with repeated-measures ANOVA.

Figure 1. Representative Raw Spectrum and Cumulative Time-Domain Lorentzian Fit in Subjects With Panic Disorder During Hyperventilation a

a The spectrum is a 6-msec TE, 2-second TR, 2-minute 15-second acquisition with 6 Hz line broadening. Individual Lorentzian fit components demonstrate the prior knowledge constraints used for the analysis. The fit residual contains noise, and some broad baseline components present at 1.5 T, demonstrating the robust frequency and selective fitting employed. Adenosine triphosphate (ATP) components are labeled gamma, alpha, and beta. PME=phosphomonoesters, pl=inorganic phosphate, PDE=fitted amplitudes of phosphodiesters, PCr=plasma creatinine.

Results

There were no group differences in gender distribution (p=0.36, Fisher’s exact test) (df=1, p=0.21), although the healthy subjects were somewhat younger than the panic disorder group (t=–1.99, df=18, p=0.06). Symptom differences between groups in the Acute Panic Inventory at baseline and after the hyperventilation procedure were demonstrated (baseline—healthy subjects: mean=0.50, SD=0.92; subjects with panic disorder: mean=4.56, SD=4.77) (t=2.77, df=18, p=0.01) (posthyperventilation—healthy subjects: mean=1.63, SD=2.06; subjects with panic disorder: mean=7.39, SD=7.19) (t=–2.54, df=18, p=0.02) and for anxiety ratings (baseline—healthy subjects: mean=0.36, SD=0.67; subjects with panic disorder: mean=1.33, SD=1.00; Mann-Whitney U=21.5, df=18, p=0.03; posthyperventilation: healthy subjects: mean=0.09, SD=0.30; subjects with panic disorder: mean=1.56, SD=1.33; Mann-Whitney U=13.5, df=18, p=0.002). No differences were found at baseline for the panic ratings (healthy subjects: mean=0.23, SD=0.75; subjects with panic disorder: mean=1.11, SD=1.83; Mann-Whitney U=37.0, df=18, p=0.18), although significant differences were present after the hyperventilation paradigm (healthy subjects: all=0; subjects with panic disorder: mean=2.33, SD=1.94; Mann-Whitney U=16.5, df=18, p=0.002). Despite greater endorsement of anxiety symptoms in the panic disorder group, no subject reported a response similar to a panic attack, nor did any subject meet DSM-IV criteria for a panic attack in response to hyperventilation.

In both groups, pCO 2 decreases were demonstrated across the hyperventilation phase for the effect of time (F=191.62, df=19, 342, p<0.001; Greenhouse-Geisser effect=0.001), with significantly different group response-by-time interaction (F=2.84, df=19, 342, p<0.001; Greenhouse-Geisser effect=0.02). The group main effect for CO 2 differences was less than significant (F=3.84, df=1, 18, p=0.07). Post hoc testing of the group-by-time effect demonstrated greater decreases in pCO 2 by scan nine in the panic disorder group (t=1.96, df=18, p=0.07) and significant decreases on the last scan of the hyperventilation period (scan 10) (t=2.72, df=18, p=0.01). In the posthyperventilation phase, pCO 2 recovery was significantly delayed in the panic disorder group on scans 11–17 (all t>2.17, df=18, p<0.04), with a difference observed on scans 18 and 19 (all t<2.01, df=18, p<0.06) ( Figure 2 ). To ensure that the delayed recovery was not simply a result of different group pCO 2 minima at the end of the hyperventilation period, data were transformed for each subject to a percent recovery value (B–R)/(B–H), where B was the mean baseline pCO 2 , R was the pCO 2 at each point in recovery, and H was the pCO 2 at the end of hyperventilation. Correction for the greater pCO 2 minimum in the panic disorder group at the end of hyperventilation with this percent recovery formula did not markedly affect pCO 2 results, with group differences observed on scans 11, 12, and 20 (all t>2.75, df=18, p<0.10) and significant differences in pCO 2 recovery for scans 13–19 (all t>2.37, df=18, p<0.03).

Figure 2. pH Means Plotted Across Baseline During Voluntary Hyperventilation and Posthyperventilation and pCO 2 Response in Subjects With Panic Disorder and Healthy Comparison Subjects a

a The pH means are plotted across baseline (five scans over 12 minutes) during voluntary hyperventilation (five scans) and posthyperventilation (10 scans over 24 minutes) by group. The pCO 2 group response is given in means. With the healthy comparison subjects model (pCO 2 -pH) used for prediction, the predicted pH curve for the panic disorder group is shown at the top, illustrating the blunted magnitude of measured alkalosis in this group.

Across the respiratory challenge, brain pH increases were demonstrated for both groups (time F=5.36, df=19, 342, p<0.001; Greenhouse-Geisser effect=0.001). However, no differential effects of group (F=1.14, df=1, 18, p=0.30) or group by time were demonstrated (F=0.75, df=19, 342, p=0.77; Greenhouse-Geisser effect=0.64). Despite the greater pCO 2 decrease during the hyperventilation phase, the panic disorder group did not demonstrate increased brain alkalosis; instead, pH values for subjects with panic disorder were not significantly different from comparison values throughout the hyperventilation protocol ( Figure 2 ). In the posthyperventilation phase, there was slower pCO 2 recovery, but pH recovery was not significantly different between the subjects with panic disorder and the healthy comparison subjects.

The discordance between pCO 2 and pH measurements in the panic disorder group led to exploratory analyses aimed to model the degree of pH blunting. With healthy comparison subjects’ pCO 2 and pH values, linear regression models were created for the hyperventilation (five scans) and posthyperventilation (10 scans) phases. Comparison group pCO 2 and pH values were entered into a linear regression for each phase, and slope and intercept terms were generated. Resultant linear regression models were as follows: hyperventilation: pH=–0.0124 * pCO 2 + 7.4178; posthyperventilation: pH=–0.0023 * pCO 2 + 7.228. pCO 2 data for subjects with panic disorder were then entered into these models for healthy comparison subjects, generating estimated pH response values for the panic disorder group. The predicted panic disorder pH curve from these models is shown in Figure 2 , illustrating the discrepancy between the estimated and measured acid-base response.

No differences were found when we compared phosphomonoesters, phosphodiesters, or inorganic phosphate amplitudes by group (all F<1.88, df=1, 18, p>0.19), time (all F<1.06, df=19, 342, p>0.39; Greenhouse-Geisser effect>0.39), or group by time (all F<0.83, df=19, 342, p>0.67; Greenhouse-Geisser effect>0.39). Similarly, no differences were found for phosphocreatine or ATP by group (all F<1.21, df=1, 18, p>0.28), time (all F<1.43, df=19, 342, p>0.11; Greenhouse-Geisser effect>0.19), or group by time (all F<1.29, df=19, 342, p>0.19; Greenhouse-Geisser effect>0.25).

Discussion

Despite similar training and feedback provided during the hyperventilation phase, the subjects with panic disorder had difficulty maintaining a hyperventilation rate that produced a pCO 2 of 20 mm Hg. This overbreathing led to more robust pCO 2 changes in the panic disorder group during the hyperventilation phase. Across this same interval, however, the magnitude of alkalosis was similar between groups. This physiological response pattern, wherein a substantially greater decrease in pCO 2 was not accompanied by a similar magnitude of alkalotic response (a pH increase), supports a model of greater alkalotic buffering in the panic disorder group. Consistent with this idea, estimating panic disorder pH response with data from the healthy comparison subjects as a model acid-base buffering system illustrates that the induced alkalosis should have been greater in the panic disorder group, reaching a maximum equal to the healthy comparison subjects at an earlier hyperventilation time point. This predicted asymptotic pH response with decreasing pCO 2 levels is similar to the pH curve observed in animal work (31) , adding support for the validity of this modeling approach and data interpretation.

In the posthyperventilation period, there was delayed pCO 2 recovery in the panic disorder group assessed with either raw or percent recovery values, but the time course of pH normalization was not substantially different between groups. Recent evidence suggests that this may reflect the insensitivity of pH as a metric to predict hyperventilation maintenance. From a study of hypoxia in rats, dichloroacetate, administered to block lactate production, markedly reduced hyperventilation (32) . If the converse is operating in subjects with panic disorder, it is possible that elevated lactate in response to hyperventilation might be driving or initiating the persistent respiratory behavior after voluntary hyperventilation cessation. Heuristically, it remains undetermined whether the exaggerated lactate response reported in prior work reflects increased production in panic disorder or whether lactate, once produced, is cleared more slowly, perhaps because of altered enzymatic activity, such as lactate dehydrogenase. This latter point has some support from the genetic literature investigating subjects sensitive to anxiety provocation by CO 2 inhalation (33) . Further work measuring lactate in parallel with brain pH at high time resolution will be necessary to clarify a mechanistic description of events.

Similar to past work, medication-treated subjects with panic disorder were studied to minimize state-specific anxiety contributions to physiological measures and to allow assessment of underlying trait features (19) . Although the panic disorder group showed increased levels of anxiety at baseline and after hyperventilation challenge in relation to comparison subjects, symptom severity was substantially lower than would be expected among untreated patients with panic disorder (24) . Future work evaluating medication-treated healthy comparison subjects or a panic group that symptomatically improved with cognitive behavior therapy will aid in evaluating what effect, if any, medication had on the measured results.

A further point to consider when we interpret these results is the rapid cerebral blood flow (CBF) reduction that occurs in response to hypocapnea (34 , 35) . Altered blood flow response to hyperventilation in subjects with panic disorder has been implicated in two studies (36 , 37) . On this point, pilot work conducted in 1998 on six of seven subjects with panic disorder who participated in the 1 H MRS investigation by Dager et al. (19) did not demonstrate differential middle cerebral artery flow velocity changes using transcranial Doppler during an identical hyperventilation challenge (unpublished data of S. Dager and K. Beach). In the present study, high energy phosphates (ATP and phosphocreatine) were assessed as an indirect indication of hypoxia. Despite the increased hypocapnea in the panic disorder group, ATP and phosphocreatine levels were not significantly altered, suggesting that this factor, per se, was not a major contributor to the observed pH results.

Further study is necessary to evaluate whether lactate increase is a direct buffering response, as we have postulated, or instead a byproduct of another process affected by the acid-base perturbation. For example, it is likely that hyperventilation-induced increases in pH alter the redox state toward glycolysis (25) . In this model, lactate increases would reflect a redox shift instead of a primary buffering process. Quantifying the time course and buffering components of such acid-base changes will be important to delineate in future work.

Whether pH and CO 2 dysregulation are also present in panic disorder subgroups not susceptible to lactate infusion (which also causes an alkalotic state) or for patients susceptible to only acidotic perturbations (e.g., CO 2 inhalation) (38) remains an interesting research question. It is also possible that subgroups of panic subjects sensitive to only alkalotic or acidotic paradigms have pH regulatory abnormalities that are specific in direction and a further subgroup sensitive to both challenge types has altered buffering in general. Future work exploiting higher field strengths to increase signal-to-noise ratio and improve the sensitivity of measuring small differences in pH response and energy metabolism will be helpful to further test and extend these observations.

Presented in part at a meeting of the Psychiatric Research Society, Park City, Utah, Feb. 11–14, 2004. Received July 23, 2004; revision received Dec. 8, 2004; accepted March 7, 2005. From the Departments of Radiology, Bioengineering, and Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Washington Medical Center, Seattle; the Brain Imaging Center, Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Harvard University, Belmont, Mass. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Friedman, Department of Radiology, 1100 N.E. 45th St., Suite 555, Seattle, WA 98105; [email protected] (e-mail).Supported by a National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression grant (to Dr. Friedman) and NIMH grants K01 MH-069848 (to Dr. Friedman) and R01 MH-50579-07 (to Dr. Dager).The authors thank Marie Domsalla for help with subject recruitment and coordination; James Tropp, Ph.D., at General Electric Medical Systems for providing the coil circuit board and help in coil development; and Blaise Frederick, Ph.D., for assistance with pulse sequence implementation. The Statistical Consulting Core at the University of Washington provided advice on statistical handling and data modeling.

References

1. Dager SR, Cowley DS, Dunner DL: Biological markers in panic states: lactate-induced panic and mitral valve prolapse. Biol Psychiatry 1987; 22:339–359Google Scholar

2. Klein DF: Delineation of two drug-responsive anxiety syndromes. Psychopharmacologia 1964; 17:397–408Google Scholar

3. Liebowitz MR, Gorman JM, Fyer AJ, Levitt M, Dillon D, Levy G, Appleby IL, Anderson S, Palij M, Davies SO, et al: Lactate provocation of panic attacks, II: biochemical and physiological findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985; 42:709–719Google Scholar

4. Alsene K, Deckert J, Sand P, de Wit H: Association between A2a receptor gene polymorphisms and caffeine-induced anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003; 28:1694–1702Google Scholar

5. Crowe RR: The genetics of panic disorder and agoraphobia. Psychiatr Dev 1985; 3:171–185Google Scholar

6. Torgersen S: Genetic factors in anxiety disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:1085–1089Google Scholar

7. Liebowitz MR, Fyer AJ, Gorman JM, Dillon D, Appleby IL, Levy G, Anderson S, Levitt M, Palij M, Davies SO, et al: Lactate provocation of panic attacks, I: clinical and behavioral findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:764–770Google Scholar

8. Pitts FN Jr, McClure JN Jr: Lactate metabolism in anxiety neurosis. N Engl J Med 1967; 277:1329–1336Google Scholar

9. Cowley DS, Dager SR, Dunner DL: The lactate infusion challenge, in Panic Disorder—Clinical, Biological, and Treatment Aspects. Edited by Asnis GM, van Praag HM. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1995, pp 206–232Google Scholar

10. Uhde TW, Bouilenger JP: Caffeine model of panic, in New Directions in Affective Disorders. Edited by Lerer I, Gershon S. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1989, pp 410–413Google Scholar

11. Woods SW, Charney DS, Loke J, Goodman WK, Redmond DE Jr, Heninger GR: Carbon dioxide sensitivity in panic anxiety: ventilatory and anxiogenic response to carbon dioxide in healthy subjects and patients with panic anxiety before and after alprazolam treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:900–909Google Scholar

12. Papp LA, Klein DF, Martinez J, Schneier F, Cole R, Liebowitz MR, Hollander E, Fyer AJ, Jordan F, Gorman JM: Diagnostic and substance specificity of carbon-dioxide-induced panic. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:250–257Google Scholar

13. Maddock RJ, Carter CS, Gietzen DW: Elevated serum lactate associated with panic attacks induced by hyperventilation. Psychiatry Res 1991; 38:301–311Google Scholar

14. Wilhelm FH, Gerlach AL, Roth WT: Slow recovery from voluntary hyperventilation in panic disorder. Psychosom Med 2001; 63:638–649Google Scholar

15. Shear MK, Fyer AJ, Ball G, Josephson S, Fitzpatrick M, Gitlin B, Frances A, Gorman J, Liebowitz M, Klein DF: Vulnerability to sodium lactate in panic disorder patients given cognitive-behavioral therapy. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148:795–797Google Scholar

16. Fyer AJ, Liebowitz MR, Gorman JM, Davies SO, Klein DF: Lactate vulnerability of remitted panic patients. Psychiatry Res 1985; 14:143–148Google Scholar

17. Maddock RJ, Mateo-Bermudez J: Elevated serum lactate following hyperventilation during glucose infusion in panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27:411–418Google Scholar

18. Adler S, Simplaceanu V, Ho C: Brain pH in acute isocapnic metabolic acidosis and hypoxia: a 31P-nuclear magnetic resonance study. Am J Physiol 1990; 258(1, part 2):F34–F40Google Scholar

19. Dager SR, Strauss WL, Marro KI, Richards TL, Metzger GD, Artru AA: Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy investigation of hyperventilation in subjects with panic disorder and comparison subjects. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:666–672Google Scholar

20. Maddock RJ: The lactic acid response to alkalosis in panic disorder: an integrative review. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci 2001; 13:22–34Google Scholar

21. Petroff OA, Prichard JW, Behar KL, Alger JR, den Hollander JA, Shulman RG: Cerebral intracellular pH by 31P nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neurology 1985; 35:781–788Google Scholar

22. Posse S, Dager SR, Richards TL, Yuan C, Ogg R, Artru AA, Muller-Gartner HW, Hayes C: In vivo measurement of regional brain metabolic response to hyperventilation using magnetic resonance: proton echo planar spectroscopic imaging (PEPSI). Magn Reson Med 1997; 37:858–865Google Scholar

23. Barker PB, Butterworth EJ, Boska MD, Nelson J, Welch KM: Magnesium and pH imaging of the human brain at 3.0 Tesla. Magn Reson Med 1999; 41:400–406Google Scholar

24. Dager SR, Friedman SD, Heide A, Layton ME, Richards T, Artru A, Strauss W, Hayes C, Posse S: Two-dimensional proton echo-planar spectroscopic imaging of brain metabolic changes during lactate-induced panic. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:70–77Google Scholar

25. Dager SR, Friedman SD, Parow A, Demopulos C, Stoll AL, Lyoo IK, Dunner DL, Renshaw PF: Brain metabolic alterations in medication-free patients with bipolar disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:450–458Google Scholar

26. Dillon DJ, Gorman JM, Liebowitz MR, Fyer AJ, Klein DF: Measurement of lactate-induced panic and anxiety. Psychiatry Res 1987; 20:97–105Google Scholar

27. Derby K, Tropp J, Hawryszko C: Design and evaluation of a novel dual-tuned resonator for spectroscopic imaging. J Magn Reson 1990; 86:645–651Google Scholar

28. Christensen JD, Kaufman MJ, Levin JM, Mendelson JH, Holman BL, Cohen BM, Renshaw PF: Abnormal cerebral metabolism in polydrug abusers during early withdrawal: a 31P MR spectroscopy study. Magn Reson Med 1996; 35:658–663Google Scholar

29. Lim KO, Pauly J, Webb P, Hurd R, Macovski A: Short TE phosphorus spectroscopy using a spin-echo pulse. Magn Reson Med 1994; 32:98–103Google Scholar

30. van den Boogaart A, Van Hecke A, Van Huffel P, Graveron-Demilly S, van Ormondt D, de Beer R: MRUI: a graphical user interface for accurate routine MRS data analysis, in Proceedings of the ESMRMB 13th Annual Meeting. Prague, 1996, p 318Google Scholar

31. Kjallquist A, Nardini M, Siesjo BK: The regulation of extra- and intracellular acid-base parameters in the rat brain during hyper- and hypocapnia. Acta Physiol Scand 1969; 76:485–494Google Scholar

32. Gargaglioni LH, Bicego KC, Steiner AA, Branco LG: Lactate as a modulator of hypoxia-induced hyperventilation. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 2003; 138:37–44Google Scholar

33. Philibert RA, Nelson JJ, Sandhu HK, Crowe RR, Coryell WH: Association of an exonic LDHA polymorphism with altered respiratory response in probands at high risk for panic disorder. Am J Med Genet 2003; 117B:11–17Google Scholar

34. Posse S, Kemna LJ, Elghahwagi B, Wiese S, Kiselev VG: Effect of graded hypo- and hypercapnia on fMRI contrast in visual cortex: quantification of T(*)(2) changes by multiecho EPI. Magn Reson Med 2001; 46:264–271Google Scholar

35. Lu H, Golay X, Pekar JJ, Van Zijl PC: Functional magnetic resonance imaging based on changes in vascular space occupancy. Magn Reson Med 2003; 50:263–274Google Scholar

36. Ball S, Shekhar A: Basilar artery response to hyperventilation in panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1603–1604Google Scholar

37. Gibbs DM: Hyperventilation-induced cerebral ischemia in panic disorder and effect of nimodipine. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:1589–1591Google Scholar

38. Gorman JM, Fyer MR, Goetz R, Askanazi J, Liebowitz MR, Fyer AJ, Kinney J, Klein DF: Ventilatory physiology of patients with panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:31–39Google Scholar