Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroimaging, 21 June 2023
Sec. Neuroimaging for Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 2 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnimg.2023.1207844

Structural and functional characterization of the locus coeruleus in young and late middle-aged individuals

Alexandre Berger1,2,3 Ekaterina Koshmanova1 Elise Beckers1,4 Roya Sharifpour1 Ilenia Paparella1 Islay Campbell1 Nasrin Mortazavi1 Fermin Balda1 Yeo-Jin Yi5,6 Laurent Lamalle1 Laurence Dricot2 Christophe Phillips1 Heidi I. L. Jacobs4,7 Puneet Talwar1 Riëm El Tahry2,8,9 Siya Sherif1 Gilles Vandewalle1*
  • 1Sleep and Chronobiology Lab, GIGA-Cyclotron Research Centre-In Vivo Imaging, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
  • 2Institute of Neuroscience (IoNS), Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Catholic University of Louvain, Brussels, Belgium
  • 3Synergia Medical SA, Mont-Saint-Guibert, Belgium
  • 4Alzheimer Centre Limburg, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
  • 5Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Department of Natural Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Otto-von-Guericke-University, Magdeburg, Germany
  • 6German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg, Germany
  • 7Department of Radiology, Gordon Center for Medical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 8Center for Refractory Epilepsy, Department of Neurology, Cliniques Universitaires Saint-Luc, Brussels, Belgium
  • 9Walloon Excellence in Life Sciences and Biotechnology (WELBIO) Department, WEL Research Institute, Wavre, Belgium

Introduction: The brainstem locus coeruleus (LC) influences a broad range of brain processes, including cognition. The so-called LC contrast is an accepted marker of the integrity of the LC that consists of a local hyperintensity on specific Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) structural images. The small size of the LC has, however, rendered its functional characterization difficult in humans, including in aging. A full characterization of the structural and functional characteristics of the LC in healthy young and late middle-aged individuals is needed to determine the potential roles of the LC in different medical conditions. Here, we wanted to determine whether the activation of the LC in a mismatch negativity task changes in aging and whether the LC functional response was associated to the LC contrast.

Methods: We used Ultra-High Field (UHF) 7-Tesla functional MRI (fMRI) to record brain response during an auditory oddball task in 53 healthy volunteers, including 34 younger (age: 22.15y ± 3.27; 29 women) and 19 late middle-aged (age: 61.05y ± 5.3; 14 women) individuals.

Results: Whole-brain analyses confirmed brain responses in the typical cortical and subcortical regions previously associated with mismatch negativity. When focusing on the brainstem, we found a significant response in the rostral part of the LC probability mask generated based on individual LC images. Although bilateral, the activation was more extensive in the left LC. Individual LC activity was not significantly different between young and late middle-aged individuals. Importantly, while the LC contrast was higher in older individuals, the functional response of the LC was not significantly associated with its contrast.

Discussion: These findings may suggest that the age-related alterations of the LC structural integrity may not be related to changes in its functional response. The results further suggest that LC responses may remain stable in healthy individuals aged 20 to 70.

Introduction

The locus coeruleus (LC) is a brainstem nucleus in the reticular formation that is gaining a lot of scientific attention. Being the main source of norepinephrine (NE) in the brain, this nucleus sends monosynaptic projections to almost all brain regions (Aston-Jones et al., 1986; Keren et al., 2009), forming the LC-NE system. The LC is involved in numerous processes including the regulation of anxiety and alertness, the gating of sleep, the detection of salient changes in the environment, and cognition over various domains (Sara, 2009). Alteration in the LC has been suggested in an array of psychiatric and neurological diseases including Alzheimer's Disease (AD) dementia (Jacobs et al., 2021), Parkinson's disease (Braak et al., 2003), rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder (García-Lorenzo et al., 2013), insomnia disorders (Van Someren, 2020), pathological anxiety (Morris et al., 2020), late-life major depression (Guinea-Izquierdo et al., 2021) or schizophrenia (Mäki-Marttunen et al., 2020).

In vivo investigations of the functions and structure of the LC have been hampered due to the small size of the nucleus, which consists of a pair of ~15 mm long cylinder-shaped nuclei of 2.5 mm diameter with a total of ~50.000 neurons in humans (Aston-Jones et al., 1986; Keren et al., 2009). Part of this difficulty was overcome using structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) sequences seemingly sensitive to the high content of neuromelanin within the LC. Using such sequences, the LC appears hyperintense and can be isolated from its surrounding tissues (Kelberman et al., 2020). The recent development of Ultra-High Field (UHF) 7-Tesla (7T) MRI further permitted submillimeter higher resolution and higher signal-to-noise ratio for imaging the LC (Priovoulos et al., 2018). The biophysical origin of the LC contrast and the significance of its variations remain debated (Priovoulos et al., 2020; Galgani et al., 2021). The consensus is, however, that it is related to the structural integrity of the cells constituting the LC. Studies assessing LC functional responses remain scarce, likely because of the difficulty to reliably detect LC activation using functional MRI (fMRI) in humans.

In the context of healthy aging, studies reported that the LC contrast increases in adulthood up to about 60 years to decline thereafter (Shibata et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2019; Jacobs et al., 2021). The variability of the LC contrast in aging may be related to the presence of AD pathology, altering the structural integrity of the LC (Jacobs et al., 2021). A recent in vivo 3T fMRI study that used a visual novelty paradigm in humans further reported that lower activity and functional connectivity of the LC were associated with amyloid-related cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired older individuals (Prokopiou et al., 2022). However, whether LC functional response changes with healthy aging is not established. Likewise, whether age-related changes in LC contrast are associated with a detectable change in its functional response has not been investigated.

Here, we first tested whether the functional response of the LC during an auditory oddball task—a robust paradigm used to recruit the LC (Murphy et al., 2014)—differed between young and late middle-aged healthy individuals. Based on previous electrophysiology and fMRI studies that reported a reduced brain response to oddball events in aging (Juckel et al., 2012; van Dinteren et al., 2014), we expect a more prominent response in the LC of younger adults. We further investigated whether the LC response was related to the LC integrity, as assessed through its contrast, to determine if a structural-functional relationship may exist in the nucleus. Overall, this work aims to further our current knowledge about the effect of healthy aging on LC physiology and anatomy. A better functional and structural characterization of this nucleus in healthy individuals could help to detect abnormal features in different medical conditions. This could help to improve the detection of LC-related disorders and diseases in young and late middle-aged individuals, better characterize their evolution and potentially help to develop therapies that are targeting the LC-NE system.

Materials and methods

Participants

A sample of 53 healthy participants of both sexes, composed of 34 healthy young (age: 22.15 ± 3.27 years, 29 women) and 19 late middle-aged (age: 61.05 ± 5.3 years, 14 women) individuals were included in this study. A summary of the demographic data can be found in Table 1. This study was approved by the faculty-hospital Ethics Committee of the University of Liège. All participants provided their written informed consent and received financial compensation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the study samples.

The exclusion criteria were as follows: history of major neurologic or psychiatric disease or stroke; recent history of depression and anxiety (<5 years); sleep disorder; use of any medication affecting the central nervous system; smoking; excessive alcohol (>14 units/week) or caffeine (>5 cups/day) consumption; night shift work in the past 6 months; travels in a different time zone during the last 2 months; Body Mass Index (BMI) ≤18 and ≥29 (for the older participants) and ≥25 (for the younger participants); clinical symptoms of cognitive impairment for older subjects (Mattis Dementia Rating Scale score <130; Mini-Mental State Examination score <27) (Coblentz et al., 1973; Folstein et al., 1975) and MRI contraindications. Due to miscalculation at screening, one older participant had a BMI of 30.9 and one of the younger participants had a BMI of 28.4. Since their BMI did not deviate substantially from the criteria and that BMI was used as a covariate in our statistical models, these participants were included in the analyses. Depression, anxiety, and sleep quality were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1988b), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck et al., 1988a) and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (Buysse et al., 1988), respectively.

The LC-NE system has been recognized as a neuromodulator during sleep (Osorio-Forero et al., 2022). Moreover, knowing that the LC exhibits a circadian rhythm and is involved in the regulation of the sleep-wake cycle (Gonzalez and Aston-Jones, 2006), the younger participants were requested to maintain a loose fixed sleep-wake schedule (±1 h) for 1 week before fMRI acquisitions. This instruction was intended to reduce prior sleep deprivation and favor similar circadian entrainment across participants while keeping realistic daily life conditions. Adherence to the schedule was verified using a wrist actimetry device (AX3, Axivity Ltd, Newcastle, UK). Older participants were requested to avoid unusual late sleep time for 3 days prior to their participation. Adherence was verified using sleep diaries.

FMRI recordings were completed in the morning, 2–3 h after wake-up time to control for time-of-day effects. Prior to entering the MRI scanner, all participants were maintained in dim light (10 lux) for at least 45 min during which they received instructions about the following MRI sessions. The fMRI sessions consisted of a 10 min visual task followed by a 10 min auditory oddball task. The present paper only deals with the auditory task. Structural MRI data were acquired in a separate MRI session completed within 1 week before or after the fMRI session.

Auditory oddball task

The task consisted of rare deviant target tones (1,000 Hz sinusoidal waves, 100 ms) composing 20% of the tones that were pseudorandomly interleaved within a stream of standard stimuli (500 Hz sinusoidal waves, 100 ms). The task included 270 auditory stimuli in total (54 target tones). Auditory stimuli were delivered to the participants with MRI-compatible headphones (Sensimetrics, Malden, MA). The interstimulus interval was set to 2,000 ms. Participants were instructed to press with the right index finger on an MRI-compatible keyboard (Current Designs, Philadelphia, PA) as quickly as possible at the appearance of target sounds. The experimental paradigm was designed using OpenSesame 3.2.8 (Mathôt et al., 2012). The MRI session started with a short volume calibration session to ensure an optimal perception of the stimuli.

MRI data acquisitions

MRI data were acquired using a MAGNETOM Terra 7T MRI system (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany), with a single-channel transmit and 32 channel receive head coil (1TX/32RX Head Coil, Nova Medical, Wilmington, MA). To reduce dielectric artifacts and homogenize the magnetic field of Radio Frequency (RF) pulses, dielectric pads were placed between the head of the participants and the coil (Multiwave Imaging, Marseille, France). Foam pads were used to stabilize the head of the participants and limit the motion during the acquisition.

BOLD fMRI data were acquired during the task, using the CMRR multi-band (MB) Gradient-Recalled Echo—Echo-Planar Imaging (GRE-EPI) sequence: TR = 2,340 ms, TE = 24 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 160 × 160, 86 axial 1.4 mm-thick slices, MB acceleration factor = 2, GRAPPA acceleration factor = 3, voxel size = (1.4 × 1.4 × 1.4)mm3. The cardiac pulse and the respiratory movement were recorded concomitantly using a pulse oximeter and a breathing belt (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany), respectively. The fMRI acquisition was followed by a 2D GRE field mapping sequence to assess B0 inhomogeneity with the following parameters: TR = 5.2 ms, TEs = 2.26 ms and 3.28 ms, FA = 15°, bandwidth = 737 Hz/pixel, matrix size = 96 × 128, 96 axial 2.0 mm-thick slices, voxel size = (2 × 2 × 2) mm3, acquisition time = 1:38 min.

A Magnetization-Prepared with 2 RApid Gradient Echoes (MP2RAGE) sequence was used to acquire T1-weighted anatomical images: TR = 4,300 ms, TE = 1.98 ms, FA1/FA2 = 5°/6°, TI1/TI2 = 940 ms/2,830 ms, bandwidth = 240 Hz/pixel, matrix size = 296 × 256, 224 axial 0.75 mm-thick slices, GRAPPA acceleration factor = 3, voxel size = (0.75 × 0.75 × 0.75) mm3, acquisition time = 9:03 min (Marques and Gruetter, 2013). The LC-specific sequence consisted of a 3D high-resolution Magnetization Transfer-weighted Turbo-FLash (MT-TFL) sequence with the following parameters: TR = 400 ms, TE = 2.55 ms, FA = 8°, bandwidth = 300 Hz/pixel, matrix size = 480 × 480, number of averages = 2, turbo factor = 54, MTC pulses = 20, MTC FA = 260°, MTC RF duration = 10,000 μs, MTC Inter RF delay = 4,000 μs, MTC offset = 2,000 Hz, voxel size = (0.4 × 0.4 × 0.5) mm3, acquisition time = 8:13 min. Sixty axial slices were acquired and centered for the acquisitions perpendicularly to the rhomboid fossa (i.e., the floor of the fourth ventricle located on the dorsal surface of the pons; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Realigned LC slab on the anatomical T1-weighted image and (B) LC contrast on the MT-TFL image.

MRI data pre-processing

EPI images were realigned and unwarped using the Statistical Parametric Mapping toolbox (SPM12, https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/download/). Although this procedure should in principle control for all movement-related bias, only volunteers with head movement of <3 mm and <3° were included (no subject had to be removed using this criteria). In order to make sure that a possible head motion difference between young and late middle-aged individuals did not bias brain-related activations, head motion was compared between the two groups, as an image quality control. For each movement parameter (translation in x, y, z, and rotations: pitch, roll, yaw), a metric characterizing the movement during the acquisition was computed as (for example for the x-translation):

overall xtranslation=  volume 1volume end|xtranslation|

Each movement parameter was statistically compared between young and late middle-aged individuals using a two-sample t-test. No difference between the two groups was observed (translation in x: p = 0.14, y: p = 0.39, z: p = 0.62; pitch: p = 0.54, roll: p = 0.69, yaw: 0.61). No difference was observed when computing the mean translation and rotation of each participant (translation: p = 0.62 and rotation: p = 0.95).

EPI images underwent brain extraction using “BET” from the FMRIB Software Library suite (FSL, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki) and the final images were spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel characterized by a full width at half maximum of 3 mm.

The background noise in MP2RAGE images was removed using an extension of SPM12 (extension: https://github.com/benoitberanger/mp2rage) (O'Brien et al., 2014). The denoised image was then automatically reoriented using the “spm-auto-reorient” SPM function and corrected for intensity non-uniformity using the bias correction method implemented in SPM. Brain extraction was then conducted on the denoised-reoriented-biased-corrected image using both the Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs, http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/) (Avants et al., 2011) with the “antsBrainExtraction” function and the RObust Brain EXtraction tool (ROBEX, https://www.nitrc.org/projects/robex) (Iglesias et al., 2011). The method yielding to the best extraction for each individual as assessed by visual inspection, was used for subsequent steps. A whole-brain T1 group template was created using ANTs, based on preprocessed MP2RAGE images of all subjects except for one, the MP2RAGE image of whom was not adapted due to a bad positioning of the slices during the acquisitions. Finally, the preprocessed MP2RAGE image of each subject was normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (MNI152—with a 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 image resolution). The purpose of using a template that is specific to our dataset, was to improve the registration into the MNI space using an intermediate space. The transformation parameters obtained from normalization were later used for registering first-level statistical maps into the MNI space to conduct group-level fMRI analyses. While the LC reactivity was extracted in the native space of the subject (see Statistical analyses section), a group-analysis was conducted to evaluate the general oddball effects.

In order to extract LC contrast, T1 structural images in the native space of the subject (after removing the background noise) were upsampled by a factor 2 [(0.375 × 0.375 × 0.375) mm3] to avoid losing in-plane resolution when registering the LC slab to the T1 image. The upsampling was done using the “nii_scale_dims” function from an extension of SPM12 (extension: https://github.com/rordenlab/spmScripts). The complete LC contrast extraction was done in the native space of the subject. The MT-TFL image of each subject was registered with the whole brain upsampled T1 image by means of a two-step process: (i) a rough manual registration to extract the parameters for an initial transformation using ITK-SNAP (Yushkevich et al., 2016), and (ii) an automatic affine registration based on the initial transformation parameters, using ANTs (Figure 1A). MT-TFL data of one subject was not usable, due to an excessive motion of the participant, leading to a registration failure. The LC appearing hyperintense on registered MT-TFL images (Figure 1B) was manually delineated by two expert raters and the intersection of the LC masks of the two raters was computed as the final LC mask for each individual. The LC mask was skeletonized by only keeping the voxel with the highest intensity in each axial slice. Based on the skeletonized LC mask, the LC contrast was computed after normalization of each LC slice intensity to a slice-corresponding 2D reference region [a 15 × 15 voxels region, corresponding to a (5.5 × 5.5) mm2 square region] situated anteriorly (and centrally) in the pons, in the pontine tegmentum. For example, the left LC contrast was defined as:

Contrast LCLeft=(LCLeft,imean(2D ponsi)mean(2D ponsi)) 

where: i is the slice index along the (left) LC; LCLeft,i is the intensity of the voxel with the highest intensity in the axial slice with index i; mean (2D ponsi) represents the mean intensity in the 2D reference region corresponding to the axial slice with index i.

The LC contrast was computed as the mean LC contrast between the left and right LC. Individual skeletonized LC masks were used for extracting the LC activity during the oddball task in the structural space of the subject (statistical output were identical when using the full individual LC masks rather than the skeletonized masks).

In order to investigate the activation of the LC at the group-level, an LC probabilistic template was created. The LC mask of each volunteer was normalized to the structural group template, and then to the MNI (MNI152−1 × 1 × 1 mm3). This was done using the “antsApplyTransforms” ANTs command, with the transformation parameters estimated (i) when registering the subject-specific MP2RAGE image to the structural template, and (ii) the transformation parameters estimated when registering the structural template to the MNI. The final LC probabilistic template was created as the sum of all masks divided by the number of subjects included in the analysis.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using SPM12. A high-pass filter with a 128 s cutoff was applied to remove slow signal drifts. The timing vector with the appearance of the target tones was convolved with the canonical Hemodynamic Response Function (HRF) to model the event-related response and was used as the main condition in a General Linear Model (GLM). The PhysIO Toolbox (https://www.tnu.ethz.ch/en/software/tapas/documentations/physio-toolbox) was used to compute physiology-related voxel-wise signal fluctuations based on respiratory and cardiac pulsation data (Kasper et al., 2017), that was available in 48 volunteers (physiological data was not available for five volunteers). The Fourier expansion of cardiac and respiratory phase computed with the toolbox as well as the realignment parameters were used as multiple regressors of no interest in the GLM. To avoid any registration-induced error, the first-level statistical analysis was conducted in the native space of the subject.

The mean functional image was registered to the MP2RAGE image to extract the corresponding transformation matrix used to register the first-level statistical map of each subject to the structural image. Therefore, for all subjects, statistical maps corresponding to the appearance of target sounds were registered to the structural native space, normalized to the group template space and then to the MNI space. A second-level analysis was then conducted in the MNI space (to report coordinates of activation clusters), where age, sex and BMI were used as covariates. Whole brain activation was first assessed following voxel-level Family-Wise Error (FWE) correction based on random field theory for p < 0.05. For the sake of simplicity, only clusters with a size of at least 20 voxels were reported (an extensive table without minimum cluster size and all significant voxel cluster is available upon request to the corresponding author). Structures showing peak activation foci were identified using the Harvard-Oxford Subcortical and Cortical structural Atlases (Desikan et al., 2006). The probabilistic mask of the LC was then used to assess specific activation of the LC. Due to the small size of the nucleus, LC activation was not expected to survive stringent whole brain FWE correction. Therefore, a small-volume correction using the LC template was conducted using SPM12 to report voxel-level FWE-corrected results within the LC mask.

REX Toolbox (https://web.mit.edu/swg/software.htm) was then used to extract the activity estimates (betas) associated with the appearance of the target sounds in the skeletonized LC mask of each subject, in the native space to avoid possible registration errors that may occur with normalization (Duff et al., 2007). Therefore, the LC mask was resampled to the dimension of functional images when extracting the LC reactivity with the REX Toolbox. This procedure ensured that any potential displacement and bias introduced by the normalization step into the common MNI space did not affect individual activity estimates. Statistical analyses using these activity estimates were performed in Rstudio (version 2022.07.1; https://www.rstudio.com/). For all models, a multivariate linear modeling approach was used, using sex, BMI and education as covariates. LC response and contrast followed Gaussian distributions. The three models of interest investigated in the present study were: (i) LCcontrast ~ age + covariates, (ii) LCresponse ~ age + covariates and (iii) LCresponse ~ LCcontrast + age + (LCcontrast × age) + covariates. The first and second models were designed to assess age-related changes in LC contrast and LC response, respectively. The third model was intended to seek a relationship between functional response and LC contrast. We computed a prior sensitivity analysis to get an indication of the minimum detectable effect size in our main analyses given our sample size. According to G*Power 3 (version 3.1.9.4) (Faul et al., 2009), taking into account a power of 0.8, an error rate α of 0.05, and a sample size of 52 (33 + 19), we could detect medium effect sizes r >0.33 (one-sided; absolute values; CI: 0.06–0.55; R2 > 0.11, R2 CI: 0.003–0.3) within a linear multiple regression framework including one tested predictor (LC activity) and three/four covariates (age, sex, BMI, and LC-contrast where relevant).

Results

The LC contrast across the entire length of the nucleus was extracted for each individual based on MT-TFL images. The multivariate linear model using the LC contrast averaged over both LCs as dependent variable found a significant main effect of age [p = 0.038, t = 2.13, 95% CI (3.45e−05, 0.0013)], but no main effect for BMI (p = 0.456), sex (p = 0.565) and education (p = 0.943; Figure 2A). When adding age-group in addition to age to the model to take into account the fact that age was not truly continuous in our sample, these statistical outputs remain significant [main effect of age: p = 0.012, t = 2.6, 95% CI (7.4e−4, 5.8e−3); main effect of age-group: p = 0.035, t = −2.16, 95% CI (−0.2, −0.007)]. Boxplots of the LC contrast in young and late middle-aged individuals are shown in Figure 2B. The same model was then computed for the right LC contrast [p = 0.053, t = 1.98, 95% CI (−1.06e−5, 0.001)] and the left LC contrast [p = 0.15, t = 1.44, 95% CI (−0.0002, 0.001)], separately. We further explored potential differences in LC contrast in late middle-aged individuals aged 60 or less and 61 or more in a multivariate linear model similar to the preceding and found no main effect of age subgroups (p = 0.16). Boxplots of the LC contrast in subgroups of the late-middle aged cohort are shown in Figure 2C.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. LC contrast variation with age. (A) Linear regression plot of LC contrast and age. Solid line: regression line; Dashed lines: 95% confidence interval; (B) LC contrast in young and older individuals and (C) LC contrast in subgroups of the older cohort (<61 and >61 years). The horizontal line at the mid-point of the data represents the median and the colored box (without the whiskers) represents the inter-quartile range (50% of observations).

During the fMRI recordings, the participants completed the task correctly (mean accuracy: 97.1%, SD: 10.4%). FMRI data analyses over the entire brain showed that the target tones were associated with a significant greater BOLD signal in a wide set of areas (Figure 3). At the cortical level, significant activation foci (pFWE < 0.05) were detected bilaterally over the cerebellum, the posterior cingulate gyrus, the insular cortex, the precuneus, the middle temporal gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus, the frontal pole, and unilaterally over the anterior cingulate gyrus (left), the planum polare (left), the lateral occipital cortex (right), the cuneal cortex (left), the superior frontal gyrus (left), the precentral gyrus (left) and the lingual gyrus (right; Figure 3A). At the subcortical level, a significant activation was detected bilaterally over the thalamus and the caudate. These results are in line with the reported neural correlates of mismatch negativity tasks and support the validity of our procedure (Menon et al., 1997; Linden et al., 1999; Kiehl et al., 2001a,b; Brázdil et al., 2005). A detailed list of brain activations highlighted using a stringent statistical threshold controlling for multiple comparison is provided in the Supplementary material and may serve as a reference for future 7T MRI investigations using the same task (Supplementary Table S1). No significant activation difference was observed between healthy young and late-middle-aged individuals (for a statistical threshold pFWE < 0.05).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Whole-brain and LC response to the target sounds during the auditory oddball task. Sagittal, coronal, and axial views [MNI coordinates: (−3 −15 7)]. The legend shows the t-values associated with the color maps. The images are shown with the radiological convention for the orientation. (A) Whole-brain results using significance for a threshold of p < 0.05 FWE-corrected (p < 3.34e−8 uncorrected; t > 6.35), and a minimum cluster size of 20 voxels. Refer to Supplementary Table S1 for detailed list of coordinates. (B) Same results displayed using an uncorrected threshold of p < 0.001 (t > 3.26). Insets at the bottom show the LC probabilistic template and the significant activation detected within this mask (p < 0.05 FWE-corrected within LC mask). Refer to Supplementary Table S2 for detailed coordinates.

We then focused on the LC. Given the small size of the structure we did not expect that an activation would survive the conservative pFWE < 0.05 statistical threshold over the whole brain. Correction for multiple comparisons pFWE < 0.05 was rather considered within the study-specific probabilistic template of the LC in the MNI space. Following this procedure, five significant local peak intensity clusters were detected in the rostral part of the LC (Table 2). Although a significant activation was detected bilaterally, the activation clusters were larger over the rostral left LC (Figure 3B).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Activation foci within the LC at the appearance of target sounds, for a statistical threshold of p < 0.05 FWE-corrected after small-volume correction using the LC template as search region.

Further analyses were conducted in the structural native space of each subject, where the individual LC activity was extracted. The multivariate linear model for the bilaterally averaged estimates of the LC activity did not yield significant main effects for age [p = 0.57, t = 0.57, 95% CI (−0.010, 0.019)], BMI (p = 0.13), sex (p = 0.49) or education (p = 0.7). Boxplots of the LC reactivity in young and late middle-aged individuals are shown in Figure 4A. Similar statistical outputs were obtained when computing the same model separately with the activity estimate of the left and right LC [main effects for age: left LC, p = 0.92, t = 0.01, 95% CI (−0.017, 0.017), right LC, p = 0.3, t = 1.05, 95% CI (−8e−3, 0.027)] or when adding age group to the model (main effect for age and group: left LC, p > 0.3, right LC, p > 0.5). We then used a multivariate linear model to assess the link between LC activity, as dependent variable, and LC contrast.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) LC reactivity in young and older individuals. The horizontal line at the mid-point of the data represents the median and the colored box (without the whiskers) represents the inter-quartile range (50% of observations). (B) Association between LC activation and LC contrast. Linear regression plot of LC activity and LC contrast. Solid line: regression line; Dashed lines: 95% confidence interval.

The model did not reveal a significant main effect of LC contrast [p = 0.77, t = −0.29, 95% CI (−22.22, 16.64)], nor an LC contrast by age interaction [p = 0.77, t = 0.77, 95% CI (−0.33, 0.44)] on the LC response (Figure 4B). Similar statistical outputs were obtained when computing the same model separately with the activity estimate and contrast of the left and right LC, respectively [main effects of LC contrast − left LC: p = 0.79, t = 0.27, 95% CI (−12.05, 15.73); right LC: p = 0.46, t = 0.74, 95% CI (−9.1, 19.72)], or when adding age group to the model (main effect for age and group: left LC, p > 0.4, right LC, p > 0.3).

Discussion

A better characterization of structural and functional characteristics of the LC in healthy individuals is critical (i) to detect abnormal early features occurring in an array of diseases and disorders affecting the LC, (ii) to characterize their possible evolution, and (iii) potentially help in the development of therapies targeting the LC-NE system. We assessed the LC functional response and LC contrast in a cohort of 53 healthy individuals aged 18–30 and 50–70 years using a UHF 7T MRI system. We did not find an association between age and LC response, as assessed during an auditory oddball task. While we found the expected age-related increase in LC contrast, this accepted marker of LC integrity was not significantly related to its functional response.

Although widely studied, the exact source of the LC contrast as extracted using specific MRI sequences remains debated (Galgani et al., 2021). It was hypothesized that LC contrast was related to the progressive increase in neuromelanin that accumulates inside the cell bodies of noradrenergic neurons, as confirmed in post-mortem studies (Fedorow et al., 2006; Zucca et al., 2006). Histology combined with post-mortem 7T MRI acquisitions, revealed that regions with T1-hyperintensities within the LC colocalized with the presence of neuromelanin-rich neurons (Keren et al., 2015) such that LC contrast might reflect the density of neuromelanin-containing noradrenergic neurons within the LC (Betts et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2019). Recent investigations further showed that the LC contrast obtained using a T1-weighted MRI sequence with magnetization transfer was still detectable in mice genetically engineered to have about 70% fewer LC cells (Watanabe et al., 2019). Therefore, the LC contrast may not be directly linked to the accumulation of neuromelanin itself, but rather to the specific microstructure of noradrenergic neurons (Watanabe et al., 2019). Overall, while the LC contrast is deemed to reflect the integrity of the LC (i.e., its neuronal density), lipid accumulation and inflammation have also been suspected to influence the LC contrast (Priovoulos et al., 2020).

In line with the literature, an expected age-related difference in LC contrast was observed in the present study (Clewett et al., 2016; Betts et al., 2017). A previous study hypothesized that the increased LC contrast with age may be linked to a related shrinkage of cells within the LC (Liu et al., 2019). However, a study using staining methods also indicated that the volume of the LC and its neuron population were not affected by normal aging (age range: 47–83 years) (Theofilas et al., 2017). We found no indication of a potential plateau or a decreased LC contrast starting after 60 years in our sample, that would be compatible with the previously reported inverted-U relationship between contrast and age with a maximum contrast found at around 60 years (Shibata et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2019). This may be due to a lack of sensitivity as our sample of late middle-aged individuals was relatively young and small: 19 individuals in total with only 10 participants with >60 years, and a range of ages 50–70 years. This may also come in part from the stringent exclusion criteria which led to a sample of very healthy individuals in which LC integrity could presumably be better preserved. This does not mean, however, that they could not harbor presymptomatic pathologies such as tauopathy (Liu et al., 2019). For example, a lower LC contrast is related to higher tau deposition in the entorhinal cortex in cognitively unimpaired individuals (Jacobs et al., 2021). Therefore, when studying the effect of normal aging, one cannot exclude that participants included are in a prodromal phase of age-related diseases. Indeed, based on autopsy data, knowing that ~50% of individuals between 30 and 40 years present an accumulation of abnormal tau proteins within the LC (Braak and del Tredici, 2011; Braak et al., 2011), some participants in the current study could already be in a Braak stage I/II. However, a tau PET examination would be needed to determine the Braak stage in our cohort of cognitively intact participants.

The LC is known to be involved in novelty or salience detection (Sterpenich et al., 2006; Murphy et al., 2014; Krebs et al., 2018). It is reciprocally connected with the cortical salience network to enhance the processing of behaviorally important stimuli (Lee et al., 2020). The salience network is involved in high-level cognitive control to detect unexpected stimuli and reorient attention (Bouret and Sara, 2005). In the present study, a reliable LC response was detected with UHF 7T MRI system during an auditory oddball paradigm, which mimics novelty and/or salience detection. In contrast to a prior study (Murphy et al., 2014) where pupil response—an accepted output of LC phasic activity—was recorded and used as a regressor to isolate LC response during an auditory oddball task, no pupil measurement was required in the present study to extract the LC response. We detected a bilateral activation in the rostral LC—a part of the nucleus densely connected to associative regions (Jacobs et al., 2020)– that was mostly left lateralized. While there is no clear consensus on the presence of a lateralization in the LC, there is increasing evidence of regional specialization of the LC in terms of cell composition and projections (Poe et al., 2020). Previous tracing studies conducted in rats showed that rostral LC projections innervate a multitude of brain areas, including the hippocampus, the septum, the caudate-putamen (Mason and Fibiger, 1979) and the hypothalamus (Mason and Fibiger, 1979; Loughlin et al., 1986). While an intense labeling was observed in the caudal LC after injection of tracers into the thalamus, a scattered cell labeling in the rostral LC was also reported (Mason and Fibiger, 1979; Simpson et al., 1997). Although translation to humans may not be straightforward, these results support that the LC subparts are not uniform in terms of projections to the different brain areas. It is therefore plausible that the rostral activation reflects, at least in part, a true functional regional effect related to the ongoing cognitive process during the oddball task, rather than to sensitivity issues (that we cannot, however, rule out).

The fact that the LC response remained stable in late middle-aged individuals compared to young adults was not in line with our expectations. Once again, these results could be due to the very healthy and relatively young nature of our sample or the inability of fMRI to capture subtle LC response differences between individuals during an auditory oddball task. The oddball task may also be too cognitively undemanding to trigger true age-related differences. Despite this, our results suggest that the age-related increase in LC contrast does not necessarily translate in a detectable change in functional response. This observation is further reinforced by the absence of correlation between LC contrast and LC response—at least in a simple auditory mismatch negativity task. Although longitudinal data could confirm this causality, our results may support the idea that LC contrast is more sensitive to aging or takes place before functional changes. One could hypothesize that a compensation mechanism takes place to counter age-related alterations in the LC structure, as reflected by a change in LC contrast. This compensation mechanism would, however, need to scale out exactly the consequence of the structural alteration so that no difference between age groups can be detected. This cannot be ruled out though compensatory mechanisms to sustain cognition in aging most often result in higher activation or bilateralization of brain responses (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2010).

The activity of the LC neurons is known to follow tonic and phasic modes. While the tonic activity of LC neurons is known to be involved in task disengagement and search of alternative behaviors (exploration), the phasic activity during a goal-oriented task facilitates task-related behaviors in order to maximize the performance (exploitation) (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). A trade-off between these two modes allows to maximize the reward and the utility (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). Since the auditory oddball paradigm in the present study did not require the search for alternative behaviors at the expense of a task performance optimization, one could hypothesize that only the phasic activity of the LC was investigated with our protocol. The repetition time used for our acquisitions (i.e., 2.34 s) is, however, relatively long compared to the fast burstiness of LC neurons, such that tonic activity or interactions between tonic and phasic activity could contribute to our findings. However, future studies should evaluate the relationship between age-related changes in LC contrast and tonic activity. Other analyses could also probe a potential link between LC contrast and resting-state functional connectivity using the LC as a seed region (Jacobs et al., 2018).

It is worth mentioning that while linear patterns are sought in the present study, no individual between the age of 30–50 was sampled, constituting a limitation. A study with a less discretized data sample could confirm our results. Moreover, sexes of the participants were unbalanced, since only 5 males were recruited in each group (young and late-middle aged individuals). Future studies may also want to use individually tailored Hemodynamic Response Functions (HRF) to assess LC response. While the canonical HRF we used to model activity over entire brain seems suitable to model average LC response over a group of participants, individual LC responses can vary substantially across individuals (Prokopiou et al., 2022).

Given the critical involvement of the LC in cognitive and behavioral processes such as learning and memory (Kety, 1972; Sara, 1985), attention (Sara and Segal, 1991), regulation of sleep and vigilance (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981), or addiction (Aghajanian, 1978), assessing its response could be useful to evaluate the integrity of the LC-NE system in a vast array of psychiatric, neurologic, and neurodegenerative disorders. For example, alteration in the activity and/or connectivity of the LC were reported to contribute to the development of depression (Szot et al., 2016; Del Cerro et al., 2020) and the symptomatology of schizophrenia (Yamamoto et al., 2014). Aside from the early tauopathy of the LC that appears to contribute to AD (Jacobs et al., 2021), synucleinopathy contributing to LC degeneration was also detected in prodromal dementia of Lewis bodies (Hansen, 2021). Finally, concerning existing therapies targeting the LC-NE system, it was suggested that the functional integrity of this system may be linked to responsiveness to Vagus Nerve Stimulation in patients with inoperable drug-resistant epilepsy (De Taeye et al., 2014; Hödl et al., 2020; Berger et al., 2021). In the context of healthy aging, our results indicate that, while LC contrast changes with age, LC responsiveness may remain stable such that any abnormal response may constitute an early sign of an unhealthy trajectory.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Faculty-Hospital Ethics Committee of the University of Liège. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

GV, EK, and SS designed the research. AB, EK, and NM acquired the data and analyzed the data (supervised by GV). AB and EK wrote the paper. All authors provided valuable insights while acquiring, interpreting, discussing the data, edited, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Fonds National de la Recherche Scientifique (FRS-FNRS, T.0242.19 and J.0222.20). Action de Recherche Concertée—Fédération Wallonie-Bruxelles (ARC SLEEPDEM 17/27-09), Fondation Recherche Alzheimer (SAO-FRA 2019/0025), University of Liège (ULiège), European Regional Development Fund (Biomed-Hub). AB was supported by Synergia Medial SA and the Walloon Region (Industrial Doctorate Program, convention no. 8193). EB was supported by the Maastricht University—Liège University Imaging Valley. RS and FB are supported by the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 860613. EK, IP, IC, NM, CP, and GV were supported by the FRS-FNRS. SS was supported by ULiège-Valeo Innovation Chair and Siemens Healthineers. RE was funded by the Walloon Excellence in Life Sciences and Biotechnology (WELBIO) department of the WEL Research Institute (X.2001.22) and the Queen Elisabeth Medical Foundation (QEMF). None of the funding sources was involved in the study design, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Acknowledgments

We thank E. Lambot, C. Hagelstein, B Lauricella, P. Hawotte, A. Claes, B. Herbillon, P. Maquet, C. Bastin, F. Collette, E. Salmon, M. Bahri, N. Belly, and G. Hammad for their help in the different steps of the project.

Conflict of interest

AB was employed by Synergia Medical SA. GV, PT, HJ, CP, and LD declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnimg.2023.1207844/full#supplementary-material

References

Aghajanian, G. K. (1978). Tolerance of locus coeruleus neurones to morphine and suppression of withdrawal response by clonidine. Nature 276, 186–188. doi: 10.1038/276186a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Bloom, F. E. (1981). Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. J. Neurosci. 1, 876–886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-08-00876.1981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., Ennis, M., Pieribone, V. A., Nickell, W. T., and Shipley, M. T. (1986). The brain nucleus locus coeruleus: restricted afferent control of a broad efferent network. Science 234, 734–737. doi: 10.1126/science.3775363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avants, B. B., Tustison, N. J., Song, G., Cook, P. A., Klein, A., Gee, J. C., et al. (2011). A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54, 2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Epstein, N., and Steer, R. A. (1988a). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 893–897. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Carbin, M. C. (1988b). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 8, 77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, A., Vespa, S., Dricot, L., Dumoulin, M., Iachim, E., Doguet, P., et al. (2021). How is the norepinephrine system involved in the antiepileptic effects of vagus nerve stimulation? Front. Neurosci. 15, 790943. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.790943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betts, M. J., Cardenas-Blanco, A., Kanowski, M., Jessen, F., and Düzel, E. (2017). In vivo MRI assessment of the human locus coeruleus along its rostrocaudal extent in young and older adults. Neuroimage 163, 150–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouret, S., and Sara, S. J. (2005). Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 28, 574–582. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., and del Tredici, K. (2011). The pathological process underlying Alzheimer's disease in individuals under thirty. Acta Neuropathol. 121, 171–181. doi: 10.1007/s00401-010-0789-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., del Tredici, K., Rüb, U., de Vos, R. A. I., Jansen Steur, E. N. H., and Braak, E. (2003). Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol. Aging 24, 197–211. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00065-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., Thal, D. R., Ghebremedhin, E., and del Tredici, K. (2011). Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 Years. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 70, 960–969. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brázdil, M., Dobsík, M., Mikl, M., Hlustík, P., Daniel, P., Pazourková, M., et al. (2005). Combined event-related fMRI and intracerebral ERP study of an auditory oddball task. Neuroimage 26, 285–293. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds Ill, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1988). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–195. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clewett, D. V., Lee, T. H., Greening, S., Ponzio, A., Margalit, E., Mather, M., et al. (2016). Neuromelanin marks the spot: identifying a locus coeruleus biomarker of cognitive reserve in healthy aging. Neurobiol. Aging. 37, 117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coblentz, J. M., Mattis, S., Zingesser, L. H., Kasoff, S. S., Wisniewski, H. M., Katzman, R., et al. (1973). Presenile dementia clinical aspects and evaluation of cerebrospinal fluid dynamics. Arch. Neurol. 29, 299–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1973.00490290039003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Taeye, L., Vonck, K., van Bochove, M., Boon, P., Van Roost, D., Mollet, L., et al. (2014). The P3 event-related potential is a biomarker for the efficacy of vagus nerve stimulation in patients with epilepsy. Neurotherapeutics 11, 612–622. doi: 10.1007/s13311-014-0272-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Cerro, I., Martínez-Zalacaín, I., Guinea-Izquierdo, A., Gascón-Bayarri, J., Viñas-Diez, V., Urretavizcaya, M., et al. (2020). Locus coeruleus connectivity alterations in late-life major depressive disorder during a visual oddball task. Neuroimage Clin. 28, 102482. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., et al. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duff, E. P., Cunnington, R., and Egan, G. F. (2007). REX: response exploration for neuroimaging datasets. Neuroinformatics 5, 223–234. doi: 10.1007/s12021-007-9001-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedorow, H., Halliday, G. M., Rickert, C. H., Gerlach, M., Riederer, P., Double, K. L., et al. (2006). Evidence for specific phases in the development of human neuromelanin. Neurobiol. Aging. 27, 506–512. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and Mchugh, P. R. (1975). ‘Mini-mental state': a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galgani, A., Lombardo, F., Latta, D. D., Martini, N., Bonuccelli, U., Fornai, F., et al. (2021). Locus coeruleus magnetic resonance imaging in neurological diseases. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 21, 2. doi: 10.1007/s11910-020-01087-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Lorenzo, D., Longo-Dos Santos, C., Ewenczyk, C., Leu-Semenescu, S., Gallea, C., Quattrocchi, G., et al. (2013). The coeruleus/subcoeruleus complex in rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorders in Parkinson's disease. Brain 136, 2120–2129. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, M. M. C., and Aston-Jones, G. (2006). Circadian regulation of arousal: role of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus system and light exposure. Sleep 29, 1327–1336. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.10.1327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guinea-Izquierdo, A., Giménez, M., Martínez-Zalacaín, I., Cerro, I. D., Canal-Noguer, P., Blasco, G., et al. (2021). Lower locus coeruleus MRI intensity in patients with late-life major depression. PeerJ. 9, e10828. doi: 10.7717/peerj.10828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, N. (2021). Locus coeruleus malfunction is linked to psychopathology in prodromal dementia with lewy bodies. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13, 641101doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.641101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hödl, S., Carrette, S., Meurs, A., Carrette, E., Mertens, A., Gadeyne, S., et al. (2020). Neurophysiological investigations of drug resistant epilepsy patients treated with vagus nerve stimulation to differentiate responders from non-responders. Eur. J. Neurol. 27, 1178–1189. doi: 10.1111/ene.14270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iglesias, J. E., Liu, C. Y., Thompson, P. M., and Tu, Z. (2011). Robust brain extraction across datasets and comparison with publicly available methods. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 30, 1617–1634. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2011.2138152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, H. I., Priovoulos, N., Poser, B. A., Pagen, L. H., Ivanov, D., Verhey, F. R., et al. (2020). Dynamic behavior of the locus coeruleus during arousal-related memory processing in a multi-modal 7T fMRI paradigm. Elife 9, 1–30. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, H. I. L., Becker, J. A., Kwong, K., Engels-Domínguez, N., Prokopiou, P. C., Papp, K. V., et al. (2021). In vivo and neuropathology data support locus coeruleus integrity as indicator of Alzheimer's disease pathology and cognitive decline. Sci. Transl. Med. 13, eabj2511. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.abj2511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, H. I. L., Müller-Ehrenberg, L., Priovoulos, N., and Roebroeck, A. (2018). Curvilinear locus coeruleus functional connectivity trajectories over the adult lifespan: a 7T MRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 69, 167–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juckel, G., Karch, S., Kawohl, W., Kirsch, V., Jäger, L., Leicht, G., et al. (2012). Age effects on the P300 potential and the corresponding fMRI BOLD-signal. Neuroimage 60, 2027–2034. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasper, L., Bollmann, S., Diaconescu, A. O., Hutton, C., Heinzle, J., Iglesias, S., et al. (2017). The PhysIO toolbox for modeling physiological noise in fMRI data. J. Neurosci. Methods 276, 56–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelberman, M., Keilholz, S., and Weinshenker, D. (2020). What's that (blue) spot on my MRI? Multimodal neuroimaging of the locus coeruleus in neurodegenerative disease. Front. Neurosci. 14. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.583421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keren, N. I., Lozar, C. T., Harris, K. C., Morgan, P. S., and Eckert, M. A. (2009). In vivo mapping of the human locus coeruleus. Neuroimage 47, 1261–1267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keren, N. I., Taheri, S., Vazey, E. M., Morgan, P. S., Granholm, A.-C. E., Aston-Jones, G. S., et al. (2015). Histologic validation of locus coeruleus MRI contrast in post-mortem tissue. Neuroimage 113, 235–245. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kety, S. S. (1972). The possible role of the adrenergic systems of the cortex in learning. Res. Publ. Assoc. Res. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 50, 376–389.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., Duty, T. L., Forster, B. B., and Liddle, P. F. (2001a). An event-related fmri study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. J Psychophysiol. 15, 221–240. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.15.4.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., Duty, T. L., Forster, B. B., and Liddle, P. F. (2001b). Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 38, 133–142. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3810133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krebs, R. M., Park, H. R. P., Bombeke, K., and Boehler, C. N. (2018). Modulation of locus coeruleus activity by novel oddball stimuli. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 577–584. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9700-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. H., Kim, S. H., Katz, B., and Mather, M. (2020). The decline in intrinsic connectivity between the salience, network, and locus coeruleus in older adults: implications for distractibility. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12, 2. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, D. E., Prvulovic, D., Formisano, E., Völlinger, M., Zanella, F. E., Goebel, R., et al. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb. Cortex. 9, 815–823. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.8.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, K. Y., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Cardenas-Blanco, A., Loane, C., Berry, A. J., Betts, M. J., et al. (2019). In vivo visualization of age-related differences in the locus coeruleus. Neurobiol. Aging 74, 101–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loughlin, S. E., Foora, S. L., and Grzanna, R. (1986). Efferent projections of nucleus locus coeruleus: morphologic subpopulations have different efferent targets. Neuroscience 18, 307–319. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(86)90156-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mäki-Marttunen, V., Andreassen, O. A., and Espeseth, T. (2020). The role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 118, 298–314. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, J. P., and Gruetter, R. (2013). New developments and applications of the MP2RAGE sequence - focusing the contrast and high spatial resolution r1 mapping. PLoS ONE 8, e0069294. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, S. T., and Fibiger, H. C. (1979). Regional topography within noradrenergic locus coeruleus as revealed by retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase. J. Comp. Neurol. 187, 703–724. doi: 10.1002/cne.901870405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., and Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 44, 314–324. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V., Ford, J. M., Lim, K. O., Glover, G. H., and Pfefferbaum, A. (1997). Combined event-related fMRI and EEG evidence for temporal–parietal cortex activation during target detection. Neuroreport 8, 3029–3037. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199709290-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, L. S., McCall, J. G., Charney, D. S., and Murrough, J. W. (2020). The role of the locus coeruleus in the generation of pathological anxiety. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 4, 2398212820930321. doi: 10.1177/2398212820930321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, P. R., O'Connell, R. G., O'Sullivan, M., Robertson, I. H., and Balsters, J. H. (2014). Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4140–4154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Brien, K. R., Kober, T., Hagmann, P., Maeder, P., Marques, J., Lazeyras, F., et al. (2014). Robust T1-weighted structural brain imaging and morphometry at 7T using MP2RAGE. PLoS ONE. 9, e0099676. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osorio-Forero, A., Cherrad, N., Banterle, L., Fernandez, L. M. J., and Lüthi, A. (2022). When the locus coeruleus speaks up in sleep: recent insights, emerging perspectives. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 23, 5028. doi: 10.3390/ijms23095028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poe, G. R., Foote, S., Eschenko, O., Johansen, J. P., Bouret, S., Aston-Jones, G., et al. (2020). Locus coeruleus: a new look at the blue spot. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 644–659. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0360-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priovoulos, N., Jacobs, H. I. L., Ivanov, D., Uluda,g, K., Verhey, F. R. J., Poser, B. A., et al. (2018). High-resolution in vivo imaging of human locus coeruleus by magnetization transfer MRI at 3T and 7T. Neuroimage 168, 427–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priovoulos, N., van Boxel, S. C. J., Jacobs, H. I. L., Poser, B. A., Uludag, K., Verhey, F. R. J., et al. (2020). Unraveling the contributions to the neuromelanin-MRI contrast. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 2757–2774. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02153-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prokopiou, P. C., Engels-Domínguez, N., Papp, K. V., Scott, M. R., Schultz, A. P., Schneider, C., et al. (2022). Lower novelty-related locus coeruleus function is associated with Aβ-related cognitive decline in clinically healthy individuals. Nat. Commun. 13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28986-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 65B, 405–415. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sara, S. J. (1985). Noradrenergic modulation of selective attention: its role in memory retrieval. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 444, 178–193. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37588.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sara, S. J. (2009). The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 211–223. doi: 10.1038/nrn2573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sara, S. J., and Segal, M. (1991). Plasticity of sensory responses of locus coeruleus neurons in the behaving rat: implications for cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 88, 571–585. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63835-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shibata, E., Sasaki, M., Tohyama, K., Kanbara, Y., Otsuka, K., Ehara, S., et al. (2006). Age-related changes in locus ceruleus on neuromelanin magnetic resonance imaging at 3 Tesla. Magn. Reson. Med. Sci. 5, 197–200. doi: 10.2463/mrms.5.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, K. L., Altman, D. W., Wang, L., Kirifides, M. L., Lin, R. C.-s., and Waterhouse, B. D. (1997). Lateralization and functional organization of the locus coeruleus projection to the trigeminal somatosensory pathway in rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 385, 135–147. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19970818)385:1&lt;135::AID-CNE8&gt;3.0.CO;2-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterpenich, V., D'Argembeau, A., Desseilles, M., Balteau, E., Albouy, G., Vandewalle, G., et al. (2006). The locus ceruleus is involved in the successful retrieval of emotional memories in humans. J. Neurosci. 26, 7416–7423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1001-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szot, P., Franklin, A., Miguelez, C., Wang, Y., Vidaurrazaga, I., Ugedo, L., et al. (2016). Depressive-like behavior observed with a minimal loss of locus coeruleus (LC) neurons following administration of 6-hydroxydopamine is associated with electrophysiological changes and reversed with precursors of norepinephrine. Neuropharmacology 101, 76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theofilas, P., Ehrenberg, A. J., Dunlop, S., Alho, A. T. D., Nguy, A., Leite, R. E. P., et al. (2017). Locus coeruleus volume and cell population changes during Alzheimer's disease progression: a stereological study in human postmortem brains with potential implication for early-stage biomarker discovery. Alzheimers Dement. 13, 236–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.06.2362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dinteren, R., Arns, M., Jongsma, M. L. A., and Kessels, R. P. C. (2014). P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 9, e0087347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Someren, E. J. W. (2020). Brain mechanisms of insomnia: new perspectives on causes and consequences. Physiol. Rev. 101, 995–1046. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00046.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, T., Tan, Z., Wang, X., Martinez-Hernandez, A., and Frahm, J. (2019). Magnetic resonance imaging of noradrenergic neurons. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1609–1625. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01858-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, K. I., Shinba, T., and Yoshii, M. (2014). Psychiatric symptoms of noradrenergic dysfunction: a pathophysiological view. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 68, 1–20. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yushkevich, P. A., Gao, Y., and Gerig, G. (2016). ITK-SNAP: an interactive tool for semi-automatic segmentation of multi-modality biomedical images. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2016, 3342–3345. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2016.7591443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zucca, F. A., Bellei, C., Giannelli, S., Terreni, M. R., Gallorini, M., Rizzio, E., et al. (2006). Neuromelanin and iron in human locus coeruleus and substantia nigra during aging: consequences for neuronal vulnerability. J. Neural Transm. 113, 757–767. doi: 10.1007/s00702-006-0453-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: locus coeruleus, Ultra-High Field (7 Tesla), aging, oddball paradigm, neuromelanin

Citation: Berger A, Koshmanova E, Beckers E, Sharifpour R, Paparella I, Campbell I, Mortazavi N, Balda F, Yi Y-J, Lamalle L, Dricot L, Phillips C, Jacobs HIL, Talwar P, El Tahry R, Sherif S and Vandewalle G (2023) Structural and functional characterization of the locus coeruleus in young and late middle-aged individuals. Front. Neuroimaging 2:1207844. doi: 10.3389/fnimg.2023.1207844

Received: 18 April 2023; Accepted: 05 June 2023;
Published: 21 June 2023.

Edited by:

Kepa Paz-Alonso, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Spain

Reviewed by:

Bin Jing, Capital Medical University, China
Liu Mengxing, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Berger, Koshmanova, Beckers, Sharifpour, Paparella, Campbell, Mortazavi, Balda, Yi, Lamalle, Dricot, Phillips, Jacobs, Talwar, El Tahry, Sherif and Vandewalle. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gilles Vandewalle, gilles.vandewalle@uliege.be

These authors share senior authorship

Download