Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Respective and Combined Effects of Impairments in Sensorimotor Systems and Cognition on Gait Performance: A Population-Based Cross-Sectional Study

  • Olivier Beauchet ,

    olbeauchet@chu-angers.fr

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, Division of Geriatric Medicine, Angers University Hospital, Angers, France

  • Cyrille P. Launay,

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, Division of Geriatric Medicine, Angers University Hospital, Angers, France

  • Bruno Fantino,

    Affiliation Department of Neuroscience, Division of Geriatric Medicine, Angers University Hospital, Angers, France

  • Gilles Allali,

    Affiliations Department of Neurology, Geneva University Hospital and University of Geneva, Switzerland, Department of Neurology, Division of Cognitive & Motor Aging, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Yeshiva University, Bronx, New York, United States of America

  • Cédric Annweiler

    Affiliations Department of Neuroscience, Division of Geriatric Medicine, Angers University Hospital, Angers, France, Robarts Research Institute, Department of Medical Biophysics, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, the University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Background

Respective and combined effects of impairments in sensorimotor systems and cognition on gait performance have not been fully studied. This study aims to describe the respective effects of impairments in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition on the Timed Up & Go (TUG) scores (i.e., performed TUG [pTUG], imagined TUG [iTUG] and the time difference between these two tests [delta TUG]) in older community-dwellers; and to examine their combined effects on TUG scores.

Methods

Based on a cross-sectional design, 1792 community-dwellers (70.2±4.8 years; 53.6% female) were recruited. Gait performance was assessed using pTUG, iTUG and delta TUG. Participants were divided into healthy individuals and 15 subgroups of individuals according to the presence of impairment in one or more subsystems involved in gait control (i.e., muscle strength and/or distance vision and/or lower-limb proprioception and/or cognition [episodic memory and executive performance]). Impairment in muscle strength, distance vision and lower-limb proprioception was defined as being in the lowest tertile of performance. Impairment in cognition was defined as abnormal episodic memory and executive tests.

Results

A total of 191 (10.7%) exhibited impairment in muscle strength, 188 (10.5%) in distance vision, 302 (16.9%) in lower-limb proprioception, and 42 (2.3%) in cognition. Linear regressions showed that cognitive impairment as well as dual combinations of impairments were associated with increased pTUG (P<0.02). Impairment in lower-limb proprioception was associated with decreased iTUG (P=0.015). All combinations of impairments, except those including muscle strength and the combinations of the 4 subsystems, were associated with increased delta TUG (P<0.04).

Conclusion

Cognitive integrity is central for efficient gait control and stability, whereas lower-limb proprioception seems to be central for gait imagery.

Introduction

Gait is the medical term use to describe human locomotion [1]. Gait is a distinctive motor attribute of an individual that changes over the life span due to the combined effect of physiological aging and morbidities, all changes leading to gait disorders [24]. Gait disorders cause numerous adverse outcomes such as falls and related-injuries, disability, institutionalisation and death [27]. Gait disorders are a worldwide major issue because of their expanding prevalence that can reach 80% in oldest-old individuals [79]. More information on the early stages of gait disorders is required to better understand their origin and, thus, to propose adapted preventive and curative interventions in older adults.

Gait is a dynamic balance condition in which the body's center of gravity (COG) is located above a small base of support while it moves in the horizontal plan [10]. As long the COG is maintained over the base of support, gait is stable [11]. To maintain postural stability while walking, an individual is therefore required to actively control the movements of its COG [10,11]. Several physiological sensory and motor subsystems contribute to the dynamic postural control, the most important ones identified in older adults being muscle strength, lower-limb proprioception, vision and cognition [1,4,1214]. Age-related physiological impairment in the performance in these 4 subsystems may, therefore, modulates gait performance [1,4,12]. The impact on gait performance and gait control of the impairment of these subsystems either considered separately or in combination, has not been fully studied in older adults [12].

From the medical examination to the research settings using spatio-temporal gait analysis, several strategies of assessment have been implemented to explore gait performance and gait control [2,4,15]. Nowadays, the use of gait analysis systems is growing in clinical routine because they are user-friendly and they allow simple objective gait measurements [16,17]. In contrast, exploring the highest levels of gait control disorders is more complex in clinical practice, and limited to two alternatives: either using the dual-task paradigm [i.e., walking while simultaneously executing an attention-demanding task], or using motor imagery of gait (i.e., the mental simulation of gait without its actual execution) [2,1316]. Recent literature has highlighted the interest of the latter approach [1821], including the mental chronometry approach applied to a well-known motor test used in clinical routine and called the "Timed Up & Go" (TUG) test [1822]. This standardized basic assessment of a functional mobility for basic daily living motor activity records the time needed to stand up, to walk 3 meters, to turn back and sit down [22]. It has been reported that cognitive performance, and in particular executive functioning, contributes to the temporal correspondence between executing and imaging gait in patients with neuropsychiatric conditions like dementia, schizophrenia or multiple sclerosis [1821]. It has also been shown that older adults with cognitive impairment executed the imagined TUG test (iTUG) more quickly than they actually performed it (pTUG) [18,19]. In contrast, healthy younger adults have similar performance in both conditions [18,19]. This discrepancy in terms of performance between pTUG and iTUG, called “delta TUG”, has been interpreted as the awareness of movements and physical performance, and thus may be used as a biomaker of the disorders of the higher levels of gait control [1821].

While the specific impact of cognitive impairment on motor imagery of gait has already been established [1821], the respective effects of the impairments in sensorimotor subsystems (i.e., muscle strength, lower-limb proprioception and distance vision), as well as their interactions with cognitive impairment, have not been studied yet. We hypothesized that impairments in sensorimotor systems and cognition could affect gait performance and the motor imagery of gait. This study aims 1) to describe the respective effects of impairments in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition on the TUG scores (i.e., pTUG, iTUG, and delta TUG) in older community-dwellers without dementia; and 2) to examine their combined effects on TUG scores.

Methods

Population selection and study design

Between January 2008 and April 2012, 4192 older community-dwellers were recruited in 8 French Health Examination Centers (HEC) in Eastern France during a free and full medical examination. Sampling and data collection procedures have been described elsewhere in detail [23]. The study was based on a cross-sectional design. The exclusion criteria were: inability to understand and speak French, an acute medical illness in the past 3 months; neurological diseases such as Parkinson’s disease, cerebellar disease, myelopathy, peripheral neuropathy; major orthopedic diagnoses (e.g., osteoarthritis) involving the lumbar vertebra, pelvis or lower extremities, inability to walk 6 meters unassisted and being younger than 65 years of age. The exclusion criteria for the present analysis were: missing values, dementia, depression symptoms defined as a score of the 4-item geriatric depression scale (GDS) ≥1, institutionalization and the use of walking aids [24].

From the initial 4192 individuals, 1792 (42.8%) were included in the present analysis. Participants were separated into 16 subgroups based on their performance on handgrip strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition. A total of 384 (21.4%) participants with no impairments defined the group of healthy individuals (HI), which was subsequently used as the reference group. The remaining 1408 participants (78.6%) were divided into 15 subgroups according to the combinations of impairments in the 4 subsystems (i.e., muscle strength and/or distance vision and/or lower-limb proprioception and/or cognition). Impairments in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception were defined as being in the lowest tertile of performance. The other two tertiles combined were used to define the normal performance. A short mini-mental state examination (S-MMSE) score ≤5 combined with one or more errors made in the execution of the clock-drawing test were used to define cognitive impairment [25,26].

Clinical and Gait Assessment

Clinical assessment corresponded to a full medical examination along with collecting age, gender, and measures of height and weight. The number of drug classes taken daily was also recorded. Body mass index (BMI, in kg/m2) was calculated based on anthropometry measurements (i.e., weight in kilograms and height in meters). The maximal isometric voluntary contraction (MVC) strength of hand was measured with computerized hydraulic dynamometers (Martin Vigorimeter, Medizin Tecnik, Tutlingen, Germany). The test was performed three times with the dominant arm. The mean value of MVC of all trials was used in the present data analysis. Distance binocular vision was measured at 5 m with a standard Monoyer letter chart and score from 0 (i.e., worst performance) to 10 (i.e., best performance) [27]. Vision was assessed with corrective lenses on if needed. Lower-limb proprioception was evaluated with a graduated tuning fork placed on the tibial tuberosity measuring vibration threshold [28]. The participants were asked to indicate when the vibration stimulus was felt for the first time (perception threshold) and when this stimulus disappeared (disappearance threshold). The latter value was used in our study and ranged between 0 (i.e., worst performance) to 8 (i.e., best performance). The mean value obtained for the left and right sides was used in the present data analysis. Cognition was evaluated with two cognitive tests. First, the S-MMSE was used to explore the episodic memory [25]. Its score ranged from 0 (i.e., worst performance) to 6 (i.e., best performance) and a score ≤5 was considered as impairment in episodic memory performance [25]. Second, the clock-drawing test (CDT) was used to examine executive function [26]. A low executive performance was considered if one or more errors were made in the execution of drawing the face of the clock and/or the hands of the clock.

Regarding the gait assessment, individuals were asked to perform the TUG at their self-selected normal speed in a well-lit environment. They all completed one trial for the TUG and then followed by the imagery of TUG while sitting in a chair. The times for each condition were recorded with a stopwatch to the nearest 0.01second. Before testing, a trained evaluator gave standardized verbal instructions regarding the test procedure. Individuals were seated, allowed to use the armrests to stand up and instructed to walk three meters, turn around, walk back to the chair and sit down. The stopwatch was started on the command “ready-set-go” and stopped as the participant sat down. For the imagined condition, individuals sat in the chair and were instructed to imagine performing the TUG (iTUG) and to say “stop” out loud when they were finished. Individuals could choose to do the iTUG with their eyes opened or closed, and they were not instructed on the choice of the modality of mental imagery.

Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents

Individuals were included after having given their written informed consent for research. The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards set forth in the Helsinki Declaration (1983). Lyon Sud-Est III local Ethical Committee, France, approved the study protocol.

Statistical analysis

The participants’ characteristics were summarized using means and standard deviations or frequencies and percentages, as appropriate. Normality of data distribution was checked using a skewness-kurtosis test. As the number of observations was > 40 for each subgroup, no transformations were applied to the variables of interest. For the current analysis, delta TUG was calculated from the following formula: (Timed up & Go realized—Timed up & Go imagined / ((Timed up & Go realized—Timed up & Go imagined) /2) x100.

First, comparisons between HI, used as the reference group, and subgroups with impairments were performed using unpaired t-test or Chi-square test, as appropriate. Due to Bonferroni’s correction applied to adjust for multiple comparisons, P-values <0.004 were considered as statistically significant for between-group comparisons. Second, multiple linear regression analyses were performed to examine the association between TUG scores (i.e., pTUG, iTUG and delta TUG) used as the dependent variables and the previous 15 subgroups of individuals with impairments in subsystems (i.e., muscle strength, distance vision, lower limb proprioception and cognition) used as the independent variables adjusted on participants’ clinical characteristics (i.e., age, gender, number of drug classes daily taken and BMI). P-values less than 0.05 were considered as statistically significant for the linear regression analyses. All statistics were performed using SPSS (version 19.0; SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL).

Results

Table 1 shows the clinical characteristics of participants according to their impairment in each subsystem. The prevalence of impairment in performance of subsystems was as followed: individuals with muscle strength impairment (IMI) (n = 191, 10.7%), individuals with distance vision impairment (IVI) (n = 188, 10.5%), individuals with lower limb proprioception impairment (IPI) (n = 302, 16.9%) and individuals with cognitive impairment (ICI) (n = 42, 2.3%). Impairment in performance in a subsystem when pooling all participants with impairment, whatever its nature, was associated with increased age, gender male, a higher number of drug classes daily taken and higher pTUG and delta TUG scores compared to the HI group (all P-values <0.001). IMI were older (P<0.001), more frequently women (P<0.001) and took more drug classes (P<0.001). They had also a worse distance vision score (P = 0.001) and lower-limb proprioception score (P = 0.003), and a higher pTUG score (P<0.001) compared to HI. IVI were also older (P<0.001), had more frequently an abnormal CDT score (P<0.001) and trended to have greater pTUG score (P = 0.005) compared to HI. IPI were older (P<0.001) and more frequently women (P<0.001). They had also greater strength (P = 0.003) and delta TUG score (P<0.001) compared to HI. ICI trended to have greater pTUG (P = 0.024) and iTUG (P = 0.027) scores compared to HI.

thumbnail
Table 1. Participants' characteristics separated by impairment in the four subsystems (i.e., muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition) involved in gait control (n = 1792).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125102.t001

Fig 1 is a graphical representation of interdependence of pTUG on the x-axis and of iTUG on the y-axis. It shows that largest discrepancies between pTUG and iTUG (i.e. worse performance) were found with impairment in lower-limb proprioception following by cognitive impairment, association of impairments in proprioception and in vision or cognition, and combination of impairments in proprioception, cognition and muscle strength. In final, combination of impairments in vision, cognition and muscle strength corresponded to worse performance.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Representation of interdependence between performed Timed Up & Go test on x-axis and imagined Timed Up & Go on the y-axis among the different subgroups of participants combining or not impairment in one or more subsystem (i.e., muscle strength, distance vision, lower limb proprioception and cognition).

*: Impairment in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception was defined as being in the lowest tertile of performance. The other two tertiles combined were used to define normal performance. Combination of episodic memory impairment (i.e., short mini-mental state examination score ≤5/6) and executive impairment (i.e., one or more errors made in the execution of drawing the face of the clock and/or the hands of the clock) was used to define cognitive impairment. TUG: Timed Up & Go. pTUG: Performed Timed Up & Go. iTUG: Imagined Timed Up & Go. s: Second.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125102.g001

Figs 2, 3 and 4 show the results of multiple linear regressions exploring associations between impairment in subsystems and scores of pTUG, iTUG and delta TUG. For pTUG and when considering impairment in one subsystem only, cognitive impairment (P = 0.036) and muscle strength impairment (P = 0.021) were associated with increase in pTUG time. In contrast, all dual combinations of impairments were associated with increase in pTUG time (P<0.03). In addition, combinations of impairments in distance vision plus muscle strength plus lower-proprioception (P = 0.006) and distance vision plus lower-proprioception plus cognition (P = 0.007) were associated with increase in pTUG time. For iTUG, only impairment in lower-limb proprioception was associated with a decrease in iTUG (P = 0.015). For delta TUG, strength impairment alone or combined with another impairment in one subsystem was not significant (P>0.07). All other combinations were significantly associated with increase in delta TUG (P<0.04), except the one combining impairments in the 4 subsystems (P = 0.055).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Multiple linear regression analyses showing associations between performed timed up and go test used as dependent variables and subgroups of individuals combining or not decline in performance in different subsystems (i.e., muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition) used as independent variables adjusted on individuals’ clinical characteristics.

TUG: Timed Up & Go. pTUG: Performed Timed Up & Go. Horizontal lines are the 95% confidence intervals extending positive and negative from the beta value. Healthy individuals are used as the reference group and correspond to the vertical axis. *: Impairment in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception was defined as being in the lowest tertile of performance. The other two tertiles combined were used to define normal performance. Combination of episodic memory impairment (i.e., short mini-mental state examination score ≤5/6) and executive impairment (i.e., one or more errors made in the execution of drawing the face of the clock and/or the hands of the clock) was used to define cognitive impairment. All multiple linear regression analyses were adjusted on individuals’ clinical characteristics (i.e., age, gender, number of drug classes daily taken and body mass index).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125102.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Multiple linear regression analyses showing associations between imagined timed up and go test used as dependent variables and subgroups of individuals combining or not decline in performance in different subsystems (i.e., muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition) used as independent variables adjusted on individuals’ clinical characteristics.

TUG: Timed Up & Go. iTUG: Imagined Timed Up & Go. Healthy individuals are used as the reference group and correspond to the vertical axis. *: Impairment in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception was defined as being in the lowest tertile of performance. The other two tertiles combined were used to define normal performance. Combination of episodic memory impairment (i.e., short mini-mental state examination score ≤5/6) and executive impairment (i.e., one or more errors made in the execution of drawing the face of the clock and/or the hands of the clock) was used to define cognitive impairment. All multiple linear regression analyses were adjusted on individuals’ clinical characteristics (i.e., age, gender, number of drug classes daily taken and body mass index).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125102.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Multiple linear regression analyses showing associations between delta timed up and go test used as dependent variables and subgroups of individuals combining or not decline in performance in different subsystems (i.e., muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception and cognition) used as independent variables adjusted on individuals’ clinical characteristics.

TUG: Timed Up & Go. Delta TUG: Calculated from the following formula: [(performed Timed “Up & Go—Timed “Up & Go” imagined) / (performed Timed “Up & Go” + Timed “Up & Go” imagined) / 2] x 100. Horizontal lines are the 95% confidence intervals extending positive and negative from the beta value. Healthy individuals are used as the reference group and correspond to the vertical axis. *: Impairment in muscle strength, distance vision, lower-limb proprioception was defined as being in the lowest tertile of performance. The other two tertiles combined were used to define normal performance. Combination of episodic memory impairment (i.e., short mini-mental state examination score ≤5/6) and executive impairment (i.e., one or more errors made in the execution of drawing the face of the clock and/or the hands of the clock) was used to define cognitive impairment. All multiple linear regression analyses were adjusted on individuals’ clinical characteristics (i.e., age, gender, number of drug classes daily taken and body mass index).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0125102.g004

Discussion

Our findings show that cognitive impairment considered either separately or in combination with any other subsystem impairment, notably muscle strength, was strongly associated with greater (i.e. worse performance) pTUG and delta TUG scores. In contrast, lower-limb proprioception impairment was associated with lower (i.e. worse performance) iTUG score. We also found that impairments in all subsystems were associated with worse delta TUG score with a tendency to an increased effect when there was an accumulation of impairments, the highest impact being shown when combining cognition and muscle strength.

Our study confirms that the association between cognition and gait control in older individuals without dementia. The past decade has brought accumulating evidence that impairment in cognitive performance in demented and non-demented individuals resulted in deterioration of gait performance [2,1315]. Commonly described in later stages of dementia, worsening of gait performance has also been reported early in the progression of dementia and even at the prodromal stage of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [2934]. Furthermore, it has been shown that both memory and executive function (EF) are particularly involved on gait control, even in routine walking condition [3337]. Nowadays, evidence of an association of EF and memory with gait control comes from clinical and brain imaging studies. First, although few studies have explored the association of EF with gait performance among healthy older adults, these studies showed similar results [3336]. It has been reported that low global EF performance was associated with a low gait performance, and more precisely with low gait speed and high stride-to-stride variability [33,35,36]. Furthermore, an association between increased gait variability and decreased performances in information updating and monitoring, which is a specific subdomain of EF, has been shown in healthy older adults [34]. It was suggested that this involvement of EF could be explained by age-related changes in sensorimotor system leading to a decrement in automaticity compensated by an increased involvement of EF to properly process all sensorimotor information. In addition to these clinical data, studies reported that abnormalities (e.g., morphological and functional) of frontal lobe was related to lower gait performance in HI [3840]. Second, there is increased evidence that memory is also strongly involved in gait control in older adults. Indeed, higher episodic memory performance has been associated to higher gait speed while single and dual tasking in older adults [41]. Lower hippocampal volume has been associated with greater gait variability [39]. Recently Shimada et al. reported that greater desactivation in the white matter of hippocampus was related to greater gait variability in healthy older adults using positron emission tomography brain imaging, this result being in concordance with previous results showing that increased gait variability was associated with poorer hippocampal metabolism in a similar population [40,42]. The strong association between EF, memory and gait control in HI could be explained by the fact that the frontal lobe is a brain structure involved in simulating motor actions and that the hippocampus has an important role in the timing for the rhythmicity of locomotion and gait navigation.

Our results underscored that lower-limb proprioception was strongly related to iTUG performance. Indeed, impairment in lower-limb proprioception was associated with a decreased iTUG time. iTUG is a motor imagery test that simulates mentally an action without its actual execution. This approach was widely used to study higher-level control of action, based on mental chronometry, which is a tool in neuroscience to measure the time course of mental operations [43,44]. A close temporal correspondence between executing and imaging gait has been previously reported in healthy young subjects [45,46]. Furthermore, this close correspondence in timing the mental simulation of gait and its actual execution reflects the functioning of a cerebral network involving the primary motor cortex as well the prefrontal area BA 10 that participates in higher-order gait control [47]. Therefore, a lower time while imaging TUG compared to executing it is considered as a biomarker of disorders of the higher levels of gait control [1921]. This association between lower proprioception and decreased iTUG highlighted a strong association between simulating an action and the body perception in the space. Proprioception refers to the sense of knowing where one's body is located in space [48]. In our study we accessed lower-limb proprioception by using a graduated tuning fork placed on the tibial tuberosity, which is the best way to measure the ability to sense the static position of a joint or limb segment [48]. There is a wealth literature on the importance of proprioception feedback in the control of movements: impairment in proprioception provoking degradation in movement performance and in particular in postural movement control [49]. Impairment in proprioception function may influence cognitive performance. For instance, it has been shown that when the quality of proprioceptive feedback was reduced in older HI under a dual-task condition (i.e., maintaining of static position while performing an attention-demanding task) they sacrificed performance on the cognitive task in favour of maintaining postural stability [50,51]. The age-related central processing deficit related to proprioception feedback reported in clinical studies has been confirmed by functional brain imaging studies that showed neural correlates. For instance, hand-foot interlimb coordination has been associated with an extended network of neural activation which included secondary somatosensory area as well as sensory integration areas located in superior temporal and supramarginal gyri [52]. More recently, we found an association between higher STV and lower gray matter volume in the right parietal lobe in older HI [53]. In addition, it is now established that the parietal lobe is able to map objects perceived visually into body coordinate positions [54]. Taken together, this strong association between proprioception and gait control could explain that lower-limb proprioception impairment resulted in altered mental imagery of gait in the present study.

Our results also showed a complex interplay between impairments in locomotion subsystems. We observed a deterioration of motor imagery of gait assessed by delta TUG, when the impairment of subsystems increases, except when all impairments in equilibration systems were combined. Furthermore, worse gait performance was reported when cognition was respectively associated with impairment in strength for pTUG and impairment in lower-limb proprioception for iTUG. Interestingly, the combination of cognitive and strength impairments provoked an important deterioration of gait performance exceeding a single additional effect. Such effect could be explained by the fact that cognition and muscle strength are the most important equilibration systems involved in gait execution. It is well established that performance in realized movement strongly depends on the integrity of muscles [5052]. In contrast, the relation with cognition remains unclear. It could be suggested that the best performance in realized movement depends not only on the high peripheral ability to perform it, but also on the cognitive ability to control it. In addition, a similar reasoning may be proposed to explain the specific effect of the combination of cognitive and proprioception impairments on the internal simulation of gait. Indeed, proprioception and cognition are both key components of gait imagery.

Some limitations of our study need to be acknowledged. First, the cross-sectional design may be problematic when exploring an association between gait and impairments in locomotors subsystems compared to a prospective cohort study design. Indeed, the causality and direction of the associations should be carefully interpreted. Second, abnormal scores on the S-MMSE and the clock drawing test could be not sufficient to diagnose satisfactorily memory and executive impairments. These tests are usually used as screening tests rather than diagnostic tools in general population. A diagnosis of cognitive impairment in these two sub-domains usually requires a multidisciplinary meeting involving geriatricians, neurologists and neuropsychologists during which the results of neuropsychological assessment, medical examination, blood tests and brain imaging are discussed. Third, although we were able to control for many characteristics likely to modify the association between gait and cognitive performance, residual potential confounders might still be present in our study. In contrast, our study has a number of strengths. First, to our knowledge, this is the largest population-based study in older adults to examine the association of gait performance with equilibration systems impairments. Second, compared to previous published studies, the major potential confounders (i.e., age, gender, and BMI) were taken into account in our study. Third, all participants had a comprehensive clinical examination that allowed fine categorization of individuals into the different subgroups of impairments.

Conclusions

Cognitive impairment was associated with worse gait performance and motor imagery of gait, which suggests that cognitive integrity is central for efficient gait control and stability. Furthermore, lower-limb proprioception seems to be central for gait imagery. Combination of cognitive impairment with lower-limb proprioception and with strength affected imagination and execution of gait, respectively. These findings could be useful for the development of new preventive and curative interventions dedicated to the improvement of gait stability in older adults.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge for their contribution to this study C. Nitenberg from Health Examination Centre of Lyon, and B. Bongue, A. Colvez and N. Deville from CETAF Saint-Etienne. We are also grateful to the participants for their cooperation. We confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available but there is a restriction because of French law on availability of digital information of individuals participating in clinical research. Indeed, in France, the National Commission on Informatics and Liberty is a administrative regulatory body whose mission is to ensure that data privacy law is applied to the collection, storage, and use of personal data. Thus, data are available upon request due to this ethical restriction. Requests for the dataset can be sent to the corresponding author Olivier Beauchet, MD, PhD (olbeauchet@chu-angers.fr).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: OB CA. Performed the experiments: BF CPL. Analyzed the data: OB CPL BF GA CA. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: OB CPL BF GA CA. Wrote the paper: OB CPL BF GA CA.

References

  1. 1. Nutt JG, Marsden CD, Thompson PD. Human walking and higher-level gait disorders, particularly in the elderly. Neurology. 1993;43:268–79. pmid:8437689
  2. 2. Montero-Odasso M, Verghese J, Beauchet O, Hausdorff JM. Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:2127–2136. pmid:23110433
  3. 3. Beauchet O, Dubost V, Revel Delhom C, Berrut G, Belmin J, French Society of Geriatrics and Gerontology. How to manage recurrent falls in clinical practice: guidelines of the French Society of Geriatrics and Gerontology. J Nutr Health Aging. 201;15:79–84. pmid:21267524
  4. 4. Panel on Prevention of Falls in Older Persons, American Geriatrics Society and British Geriatrics Society. Summary of the Updated American Geriatrics Society/British Geriatrics Society clinical practice guideline for prevention of falls in older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:148–157. pmid:21226685
  5. 5. Tinetti ME, Doucette J, Claus E, Marottoli R. Risk factors for serious injury during falls by older persons in the community. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1995;43:1214–1221. pmid:7594154
  6. 6. Robertson MC, Campbell AJ, Gardner MM, Devlin N. Preventing injuries in older people by preventing falls: a meta-analysis of individual-level data. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:905–911. pmid:12028179
  7. 7. Oliver D, Hopper A, Seed P. Do hospital fall prevention programs work? A systematic review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:1679–1689. pmid:11129762
  8. 8. Snijders AH, van de Warrenburg BP, Giladi N, Bloem BR. Neurological gait disorders in elderly people: clinical approach and classification. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:63–74. pmid:17166803
  9. 9. Oh-Park M, Holtzer R, Xue X, Verghese J. Conventional and robust quantitative gait norms in community-dwelling older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58:1512–1518 pmid:20646103
  10. 10. Dubost V, Beauchet O, Manckoundia P, Herrmann F, Mourey F. Decreased trunk angular displacement during sitting down: an early feature of aging. Phys Ther. 2005;85:404–412. pmid:15842189
  11. 11. Hsiao-Wecksler ET, Katdare K, Matson J, Liu W, Lipsitz LA, Collins JJ. Predicting the dynamic postural control response from quiet-stance behavior in elderly adults. J Biomech. 2003;3:1327–1333.
  12. 12. Lord SR, Clark RD, Webster IW. Postural stability and associated physiological factors in a population of aged persons. J Gerontol. 1991;46:M69–76. pmid:2030269
  13. 13. Beauchet O, Launay CP, Allali G, Herrmann FR, Annweiler C. Gait changes with anti-dementia drugs: a prospective, open-label study combining single and dual task assessments in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Drugs Aging. 2014;31:363–372. pmid:24683126
  14. 14. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Dubost V, Allali G, Kressig RW, Bridenbaugh S, et al. Stops walking when talking: a predictor of falls in older adults? Eur J Neurol. 2009;16:786–795. pmid:19473368
  15. 15. Beauchet O, Allali G, Berrut G, Hommet C, Dubost V, Assal F. Gait analysis in demented subjects: Interests and perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2008;4:155–160. pmid:18728766
  16. 16. Kressig RW, Beauchet O, European GAITRite Network Group. Guidelines for clinical applications of spatio-temporal gait analysis in older adults. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2006;18:174–176. pmid:16702791
  17. 17. Beauchet O, Herrmann FR, Grandjean R, Dubost V, Allali G. Concurrent validity of SMTEC footswitches system for the measurement of temporal gait parameters. Gait Posture. 2008;27:156–159. pmid:17291765
  18. 18. Beauchet O, Launay CP, Sejdić E, Allali G, Annweiler C. Motor imagery of gait: a new way to detect mild cognitive impairment? J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11:66. pmid:24742021
  19. 19. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Assal F, Bridenbaugh S, Herrmann FR, Kressig RW, et al. Imagined Timed Up & Go test: a new tool to assess higher-level gait and balance disorders in older adults? J Neurol Sci. 2010;294:102–106. pmid:20444477
  20. 20. Lallart E, Jouvent R, Herrmann FR, Beauchet O, Allali G. Gait and motor imagery of gait in early schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2012;198:366–370. pmid:22445069
  21. 21. Allali G, Laidet M, Assal F, Beauchet O, Chofflon M, Armand S, et al. Adapted timed up and go: a rapid clinical test to assess gait and cognition in multiple sclerosis. Eur Neurol. 2012;67:116–120. pmid:22236807
  22. 22. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed "Up & Go": a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39:142–148. pmid:1991946
  23. 23. Rossat A, Fantino B, Nitenberg C, Annweiler C, Poujol L, Herrmann FR, et al. Risk factors for falling in community-dwelling older adults: which of them are associated with the recurrence of falls? J Nutr Health Aging. 2010;14:787–91. pmid:21085911
  24. 24. Shah A, Herbert R, Lewis S, Mahendran R, Platt J, Bhattacharyya B. Screening for depression among acutely ill geriatric inpatients with a short Geriatric Depression Scale. Age Ageing. 1997;26:217–221. pmid:9223718
  25. 25. Haubois G, de Decker L, Annweiler C, Launay C, Allali G, Herrmann FR, et al. Derivation and validation of a Short Form of the Mini-Mental State Examination for the screening of dementia in older adults with a memory complaint. Eur J Neurol. 2013;20:588–590. pmid:22913655
  26. 26. Muir SW, Beauchet O, Montero-Odasso M, Annweiler C, Fantino B, Speechley M. Association of executive function impairment, history of falls and physical performance in older adults: a cross-sectional population-based study in eastern France. J Nutr Health Aging. 2013;17:661–665. pmid:24097019
  27. 27. Lord SR, Ward JA, Williams P, Anstey KJ. Physiological factors associated with falls in older community-dwelling women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1994;42:1110–1117. pmid:7930338
  28. 28. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Verghese J, Fantino B, Herrmann FR, Allali G. Biology of gait control: vitamin D involvement. Neurology. 2011;76:1617–1722. pmid:21471466
  29. 29. Allan LM, Ballard CG, Burn DJ, Kenny RA. Prevalence and severity of gait disorders in Alzheimer's and non-Alzheimer's dementias. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:1681–1687. pmid:16181166
  30. 30. Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and the risk of dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:412–418 pmid:22987797
  31. 31. Beauchet O, Allali G, Launay C, Herrmann FR, Annweiler C. Gait variability at fast-pace walking speed: a biomarker of mild cognitive impairment? J Nutr Health Aging. 2013 17:235–239. pmid:23459976
  32. 32. Beauchet O, Allali G, Thiery S, Gautier J, Fantino B, Annweiler C. Association between high variability of gait speed and mild cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional pilot study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:1973–1974. pmid:22091517
  33. 33. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Montero-Odasso M, Fantino B, Herrmann FR, Allali G. Gait control: a specific subdomain of executive function? J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2012;9:12. pmid:22321772
  34. 34. Watson NL, Rosano C, Boudreau RM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Sutton-Tyrrell K, et al. Health ABC Study. Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010;65:1093–1100. pmid:20581339
  35. 35. Hausdorff JM, Yogev G, Springer S, Simon ES, Giladi N. Walking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive task. Exp Brain Res. 2005;164:541–548. pmid:15864565
  36. 36. Ble A, Volpato S, Zuliani G, Guralnik JM, Bandinelli S, Lauretani F, et al. Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:410–415. pmid:15743282
  37. 37. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23:329–342 pmid:18058946
  38. 38. Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, et al. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:721–733. pmid:19850077
  39. 39. Rosso AL, Olson Hunt MJ, Yang M, Brach JS, Harris TB, Newman AB, et al.; for the Health ABC study. Higher step length variability indicates lower gray matter integrity of selected regions in older adults. Gait Posture. 2014 [Epub ahead of print]
  40. 40. Shimada H, Ishii K, Ishiwata K, Oda K, Suzukawa M, Makizako H, et al. Gait adaptability and brain activity during unaccustomed treadmill walking in healthy elderly females. Gait Posture. 2013;38:203–208 pmid:23266043
  41. 41. Killane I, Donoghue OA, Savva GM, Cronin H, Kenny RA, Reilly RB. Variance between walking speed and neuropsychological test scores during three gait tasks across the irish longitudinal study on aging (TILDA) dataset. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2013;2013:6921–6924. pmid:24111336
  42. 42. Zimmerman ME, Lipton RB, Pan JW, Hetherington HP, Verghese J. MRI- and MRS-derived hippocampal correlates of quantitative locomotor function in older adults. Brain Res. 2009;1291:73–81. pmid:19631621
  43. 43. Jeannerod M, Decety J. Mental motor imagery: a window into the representational stages of action. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1995;5:727–732. pmid:8805419
  44. 44. Jeannerod M. Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:1419–1432. pmid:8584178
  45. 45. Bakker M, de Lange FP, Stevens JA, Toni I, Bloem BR. Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach. Exp Brain Res. 2007;179:497–504. pmid:17211663
  46. 46. Stevens JA. Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action. Cognition. 2005;95:329–350. pmid:15788162
  47. 47. van der Meulen M, Allali G, Rieger SW, Assal F, Vuilleumier P. The influence of individual motor imagery ability on cerebral recruitment during gait imagery. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:455–470. pmid:23015531
  48. 48. Gandevia SC, Refshauge KM, Collins DF. Proprioception: peripheral inputs and perceptual interactions. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2002;508:61–68. pmid:12171152
  49. 49. Dijkerman HC, de Haan EH. Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30:189–201. pmid:17705910
  50. 50. Doumas M, Smolders C, Krampe RT. Task prioritization in aging: effects of sensory information on concurrent posture and memory performance. Exp Brain Res. 2008;187:275–281. pmid:18273609
  51. 51. Teasdale N, Simoneau M. Attentional demands for postural control: the effects of aging and sensory reintegration. Gait Posture. 2001;14:203–210. pmid:11600323
  52. 52. Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. J Neurosci. 2008;28:91–99. pmid:18171926
  53. 53. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Celle S, Bartha R, Barthélémy JC, Roche F. Higher gait variability is associated with decreased parietal gray matter volume among healthy older adults. Brain Topogr. 2014;27:293–295. pmid:23681340
  54. 54. Gottlieb J. From thought to action: the parietal cortex as a bridge between perception, action, and cognition. Neuron. 2007;53:9–16. pmid:17196526