Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x5gtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-26T00:45:39.450Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

When are sex-specific effects really sex-specific?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 August 2015

E. H. Chin*
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, Canada
J. K. Christians
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, Canada
*
*Address for correspondence: E. H. Chin, Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, 8888 University Dr., Burnaby, B.C, Canada V5A 1S6. (Email echin@sfu.ca)

Abstract

We examined developmental programming studies that reported sex-specific effects published between 2012 and 2014, and examined whether the authors reported a statistical approach to explicitly test whether the effect of treatment differed between the sexes, for example, a sex by treatment interaction term. Less than half of the studies that reported sex-specific effects described explicitly testing whether effects were indeed sex-specific; in most cases, an effect was considered ‘sex-specific’ if it was significant in one sex but not the other. This is not a robust approach, since significance in one sex and lack of significance in the other sex does not imply a significant difference between the sexes. However, sample size often limits statistical power to detect interactions. We suggest that if the effect is significant in only one sex, but the interaction term is not significant, alternative solutions would be to present the confidence intervals for the effect size for each sex, or using Bayesian approaches to calculate the probability that the effect sizes differ between the sexes. We present a simple example of a Bayesian analysis to illustrate that this approach is reasonably easy to implement and interpret.

Type
Brief Report
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press and the International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1. Dunn, GA, Morgan, CP, Bale, TL. Sex-specificity in transgenerational epigenetic programming. Horm Behav. 2011; 59, 290295.Google Scholar
2. Aiken, CE, Ozanne, SE. Sex differences in developmental programming models. Reproduction. 2013; 145, R1R13.Google Scholar
3. Wagenmakers, EJ. A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychon Bull Rev. 2007; 14, 779804.Google Scholar
4. Stephens, PA, Buskirk, SW, Martínez del Rio, C. Inference in ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 2007; 22, 192197.Google Scholar
5. Eddy, SR. What is Bayesian statistics? Nat Biotechnol. 2004; 22, 11771178.Google Scholar
6. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria 2015. Retrieved 29 May 2015 from http://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
7. Kruschke, JK. Bayesian estimation supersedes the t test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2013; 142, 573603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed